1 Differential Object Marking in Italo-Romance

Several languages of the world display differential marking whereby specific types of arguments are flagged by particular case and/or agreement strategies. One widely discussed example involves differential object marking (DOM; Bossong 1982: 580; 1985), namely the morphosyntactic phenomenon for which, given two subsets of direct objects in a language, one is marked differently from the other (Bárány 2018: 3). According to traditional accounts, as well as recent typological approaches (cf. Aissen 2003; Carnie 2005), DOM is triggered by certain local features or individual factors of the relevant nominals. In particular, the use of DOM is generally explained as an epiphenomenon of the activation of at least three sets of semantico-syntactic properties. DOM may be a reflex of: (i) animacy, i.e. an intrinsic property of nominals (Silverstein 1976; Dixon 1979; Lazard 1984; de Swart & de Hoop 2007: 606); (ii) definiteness, i.e. the structural codification of features related to referentiality (Lyons 1999; Aissen 2003); and (iii) other properties which are subject to discourse-related requirements, such as specificity and topicality (de Swart & de Hoop 2007; Leonetti 2008; see García-García 2005 and Iemmolo 2010 for the notion of topicality). It has been argued that DOM not only defines a subset of objects on the basis of specific semantico-syntactic nominal properties, but that it also co-occurs with a high degree of transitivity (Hopper & Thomson 1980; 1982; Torrego 1998; Cennamo 2003; Næss 2004; García-García 2005 a.o.). Given the wide range of triggering factors, languages which display DOM vary considerably depending on the nature of the single features involved and their interaction. Nevertheless, the relevant licensing factors for DOM do not define clear-cut subclasses of nominals, highlighting how the distribution of a differential marking has to be assessed cross-linguistically and from a micro-variation perspective (cf. Ledgeway 2018).

In the Romance domain, the phenomenon of DOM is attested in Ibero-Romance (Campos 1999: 1529–1545; Escandell-Vidal 2009: 837ff a.o.), Romanian (Dragomirescu & Nicolae 2016: 920–923),1 several African varieties of French (Roberge 1990: 105–107, 120–121) and Sardinian (Jones 1993: 65ff; 1995; La Fauci 1997: 51–53). In these varieties DOM is generally realised through the marker a (<Lat. AD ‘to’), with the exception of Romanian where it is marked by pe (<Lat. (SU)PER ‘on’).2 DOM is also widespread across the dialects of Italy.3 The contrast in (1a–b) from Neapolitan is representative of DOM in Romance: the specific inanimate object in (1a) occurs in the unmarked form of the accusative, witness the doubling accusative clitic ’o on the verb, whereas the specific animate object in (1b) occurs in the marked accusative introduced by a and once again doubled by the accusative clitic ’o:

    1. (1)
    1. Neapolitan
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. ’o
    2. 3MSG.ACC=
    1. verette
    2. saw.1SG
    1. ’o
    2. the.MSG
    1. libbro
    2. book.MSG
    1. ‘I saw the book’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. ’o
    2. 3MSG.ACC=
    1. verette
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Mario
    2. Mario
    1. ‘I saw Mario’

1.1 Differential Object Marking in southern Calabrian varieties

Recent fieldwork investigations4 confirm previous observations on the properties of DOM in southern Italian dialects, but also highlight some new patterns which crucially reveal significant correlations between the presence of the definite determiner and the accusative marker a. In the following section, we shall assess and discuss the patterns displayed by two subsets of dialects spoken in the extreme south of Calabria, in the province of Reggio Calabria.

1.1.1 Case Study 1: DOM in Calabrese1

The system of accusative marking we describe below is attested in several southern Calabrian dialects spoken in an area roughly corresponding to the southern side of the Aspromonte,5 e.g. Bagaladi, Bova, Cardeto, Embrisi, Melito di Porto Salvo, San Lorenzo, San Pantaleone, San Roberto, Scido. We shall henceforth refer to this subset of varieties as “Calabrese1”. Significantly the area encompassed by these localities hosts several Greek-speaking villages.6

In Calabrese1 DOM surfaces on nominal expressions denoting humans. Typically, they correspond to highly referential nominals in definite and specific contexts (Abbott 2006), including DPs with overt definite determiners (2a), personal pronouns and proper names (2b), kinship terms modified by possessives (2c), DPs headed by universal and indefinite quantifiers (2d), bare demonstratives (2e) and bare quantifiers (2f) with human reference (cf. Ledgeway 2018).

    1. (2)
    1. Embrisi
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Petru mazzau
    2. Petru killed.3SG
    1. o
    2. to.the.MSG
    1. previte
    2. priest.MSG
    1. i
    2. of
    1. Messina.7
    2. Messina
    1. ‘Petru killed the priest of Messina.’
    1.  
    1. Bagaladi
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Stamattina
    2. this.morning
    1. vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. tia
    2. you
    1. no
    2. not
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Petru.
    2. Petru
    1. ‘This morning I saw you, not Pietro.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Ste
    2. this
    1. stati
    2. summer
    1. aji
    2. have.1SG
    1. ncotrari
    2. meet.INF
    1. a
    2. to
    1. to
    2. your
    1. nonna.
    2. grandma
    1. ‘This summer I will meet your grandma.’
    1.  
    1. d.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. tutti
    2. all.MPL
    1. i
    2. the.MPL
    1. figghioli.
    2. kids.MPL
    1. ‘I saw all the kids.’
    1.  
    1. Bagaladi
    1.  
    1. e.
    1. Vidisti
    2. saw.2SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. chidu?
    2. that.MSG
    1. ‘Have you seen that guy?’
    1.  
    1. Scido
    1.  
    1. f.
    1. Iu
    2. I
    1. no
    2. not
    1. vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. nudu
    2. nobody
    1. /
    2.  
    1. a
    2. to
    1. calcheduno.
    2. someboby.MSG
    1. ‘I did not see anybody.’

The evidence in (2a–f) shows that Calabrese1 DOM occurs whenever D° is lexicalised by a determiner, a pronominal determiner or a raised N.

Although DOM is generally not licensed with nominal expressions prototypically conveying indefiniteness, it does occur when the direct object refers to a specific individual (Diesing 1992; Abbott 2006). Thus, the two sentences in (3) are not interchangeable.

    1. (3)
    1. San Pantaleone
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. du
    2. two
    1. previti.
    2. priests
    1. ‘Petru killed two priests (=two non-specific priests).’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. du
    2. two
    1. previti
    2. priests
    1. chi
    2. whom
    1. canuscia
    2. knew.1SG
    1. jeu.
    2. I
    1. ‘Petru killed two priests (=two specific priests) whom I knew.’

While du previti in (3a) refers to a set of two non-specified priests, a du previti in (3b) denotes two referents which are identified and particularised in the speaker’s mind (Leonetti 2012: 296ff; Gianollo & Silvestri in press). Therefore, in Calabrese1 DOM is a syntactic reflex of the specific interpretation of DPs and it occurs with highly referential DPs, including definite DPs and indefinite DPs when they refer to human, concrete, singular and count individuals (Table 1).

Table 1

Marked vs unmarked accusative in Calabrese1.

personal pronouns > proper Ns > definite DPs > specified indefinite nominals > generic DPs
+referential -referential
marked accusative unmarked accusative

In Calabrese1 only specific animate direct objects are marked through DOM.

1.1.2 Case Study 2: DOM in Calabrese2

A different pattern of accusative marking is exhibited by a group of dialects spoken in an area including the eastern coast of southern Calabria. The relevant evidence is represented here by the distribution of DOM in the dialect of Gioiosa Ionica and in the conservative variety of San Luca. Both dialects represent the subset Calabrese2 which is characterised by the complementarity between marked accusative and the definite determiner. In particular, in Calabrese2 DOM is excluded in the presence of an overt definite determiner, as the examples in (4) show:

    1. (4)
    1. San Luca
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. gattu
    2. cat.MSG
    1. neru.8
    2. black.MSG
    1. ‘S/He killed the black cat.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. sulu
    2. only
    1. i
    2. the.MPL
    1. omini
    2. men
    1. ‘nt-â
    2. into-the.FSG
    1. chiazza.
    2. square.FSG
    1. ‘I only saw the men in the square.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Gioiosa Ionica
    1. Ammazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. frati
    2. brother
    1. i
    2. of
    1. Maria.
    2. Maria
    1. ‘S/He killed Maria’s brother.’

Direct objects introduced by a definite determiner do not bear a differential accusative marker even when they denote referents which are explicitly presupposed in the speech participants’ knowledge:

    1. (5)
    1. San Luca
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. i
    2. the.PL
    1. cotrari
    2. kids.PL
    1. fimmini
    2. female
    1. i
    2. of
    1. Riggiu
    2. Reggio
    1. chi
    2. that
    1. canusci
    2. know.2SG
    1. tu.
    2. you
    1. ‘I saw the girls from Reggio whom you know.’

Crucially, DOM in Calabrese2 is found with some of the configurations that license DOM in Calabrese1 (2 a–f), i.e. whenever the head D° is lexicalised by a pronoun or a raised N and whenever D° is empty:9

    1. (6)
    1. Gioiosa Ionica
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Stamatina
    2. this.morning
    1. nt’â
    2. in.the.FSG
    1. chiazza
    2. square
    1. no
    2. not
    1. vitti
    2. saw.1SG.PRT
    1. a
    2. to
    1. nujiu.
    2. nobody
    1. ‘This morning I saw nobody in the square.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Petru
    2. Pietro
    1. mazzau
    2. killed.3SG.PRT
    1. a
    2. to
    1. iju.
    2. that.MSG
    1. ‘Pietro killed that man.’
    1.  
    1. San Luca
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. U
    2. him.OBJ
    1. vidisti
    2. saw.2SG.PRT
    1. a
    2. to
    1. fratima?
    2. brother.my
    1. ‘Have you seen my brother?’
    1.  
    1. d.
    1. U
    2. the
    1. santu
    2. saint
    1. vitti
    2. saw.3SG.PRT
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Diu
    2. God
    1. ‘nto
    2. in.the.MSG
    1. sonnu.
    2. sleep
    1. ‘The saint saw God in his sleep.’

Among Calabrese2 dialects, the conservative variety of San Luca exhibits the requirement that proper names be preceded by an expletive determiner (Longobardi et al. 2013). Given such requirement, DPs with proper names fail to license DOM (cf. also De Angelis in press), witness the example (7a). On the contrary, in the Calabrese2 dialect of Gioiosa Ionica, which does not license expletive articles, proper names occur with the marker a (7b):

    1. (7)
    1. S. Luca
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. e
    2. and
    1. ‘a
    2. the.FSG
    1. Maria
    2. Maria
    1. e
    2. and
    1. i
    2. them.ACC.MPL=
    1. chiamai.10
    2. called.1SG
    1. ‘I saw Petro and Maria and I called them.’
    1.  
    1. Gioiosa Ionica
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. e
    2. and
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Maria
    2. Maria
    1. e
    2. and
    1. i
    2. them.ACC.MPL=
    1. chiamai.
    2. called.1SG
    1. ‘I saw Petro and Maria and I called them.’

To sum up, the two subsets of southern Calabrian varieties exhibit two different types of accusative marking: in Calabrese1 DOM marks DPs denoting human individuals, which are highly referential (i.e. denoted by pronouns, proper names or definite DPs) or, if denoted by indefinite DPs, encode specific individuals; in Calabrese2 DOM is licensed with the same set of DPs if and only if no definite determiner occurs. From a pragmatic point of view, the DPs denoting highly referential and specific entities express a presupposition, i.e. a piece of information that the speaker implicitly assumes to be already known by all discourse participants (cf. Stalnaker 1974; Schwarz 1977: 247; Enç 1991; Diesing 1992; see also Jäger 1995; Raposo & Uriagereka 1995; Leonetti 2008; Gianollo & Silvestri in press). Specifically, we adopt a pragmatic notion of presupposition (Dryer 1996: 487; Schwenter 2005; see also Larrivée 2014: 116) whereby presupposition corresponds to activated and discourse-old information, i.e. information available to both the speaker and the hearer at a given point in discourse. We therefore assume that in the varieties of southern Calabria DOM is a reflex of the semantico-pragmatic entailment of presuppositionality.

The patterns of accusative realisation of Calabrese2 exhibit peculiar properties which need to be accounted for. First of all, the direct object is never differentially marked if the definite determiner occurs (7a). Contrastingly, if the definite determiner is absent, the accusative is marked by a when conveying a presuppositional reading (8a vs 8b):

    1. (8)
    1. a.
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. vitti
    2. saw.3SG
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. nu
    2. a.MSG
    1. cotraru
    2. boy
    1. chi
    2. that
    1. canusci
    2. know.2SG
    1. tu.
    2. you
    1. [+presuppositional]
    2.  
    1. ‘Petru has seen a certain boy whom you know.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. vitti
    2. saw.3SG
    1. (*a)
    2.    to
    1. nu
    2. a.MSG
    1. cotraru.
    2. boy
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1. [–presuppositional]
    2.  
    1. ‘Petru has seen a boy.’

In what follows a structural interpretation of the accusative configurations attested in both sub-sets of Calabrese varieties is provided.

2 Interpreting Calabrese1

In Calabrese1 DOM represents the analytic realisation of structural accusative Case (Vergnaud 2008 [1977]; Chomsky 1981). Accusative is licensed when v enters into an Agree relation with an accessible DP able to value its unvalued features (Chomsky 2000; 2001), giving rise to both long-distance feature-checking operations of Case valuation and agreement. By way of illustration, consider (9a), where the v head functions as a Probe and exhibits unvalued φ-features which need to be valued in the course of the derivation. DPs that display an unvalued Case feature (uCase) are accessible Goals for the Agree operation, which makes them active Goals. In (9a), v and the direct object fulfill these requirements and establish an Agree relation.

    1. (9)
    1. a.
    1.  
    1. b.

The feature valuation is bidirectional. The Probe’s unvalued φ-features are valued by the Goal and the Goal’s unvalued Case feature is valued by the Probe (9b). Agree results in the valuation of accusative on the direct object DP and valuation of person and number on v. This operation is essential in order for the derivation not to crash. In particular, the DP must enter into the Agree relation with the CaseACC-valuing Probe, otherwise the Case Filter is violated (Vergnaud 2008 [1977]; Chomsky 1981).

We observed that in Calabrese1 the marker a obligatorily occurs whenever a pronominal D or a raised N occupy D°.11 In southern Italian dialects personal pronouns are the syntactic expression of Person (Longobardi et al. 2013). In the varieties of southern Calabria accusative case in personal pronouns must bear the marker a:

    1. (10)
    1. San Luca
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. tia
    2. you.ACC
    1. /
    2.  
    1. *tu
    2.   you.NOM
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1. [ACC]
    2.  
    1. ‘I saw you’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Arrivasti
    2. came.2SG
    1. tu
    2. you.NOM
    1. /
    2.  
    1. *tia
    2.  you.ACC
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1. [NOM]
    2.  
    1. ‘You came’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Stu
    2. this.MSG
    1. pani
    2. bread.MSG
    1. esti
    2. is
    1. pe
    2. for
    1. tia
    2. you.OBL
    1. /
    2.  
    1. *tu
    2.  you.NOM
    1. [OBL]
    2.  
    1. ‘This bread is for you’

Person is, in turn, decomposable into further Person-related features, such as context-dependent indexicals: ‘Participant’ for second person and ‘Participant’ and ‘Speaker’ for first person (Harley & Ritter 2002). Both first and second persons are also intrinsically human. The accusative of the pronouns is realised both through the marker a and the selection of the morphological non-nominative form (11a). It follows that there is a close connection between the semantic feature of first/second person, i.e. humanness, the pragmatic entailment of presuppositionality (2.1.2) and the marked accusative.12 In Calabrese1 demonstratives functioning as third person object pronouns behave like the first/second personal pronouns if denoting a human referent13 (11a), whereas they do not exhibit DOM if referring to [–human] individual (11b):

    1. (11)
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. idu
    2. him
    1. /
    2.  
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. chidu
    2. that.MSG
    1. /
    2.  
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. chistu
    2. this.MSG
    1. [+human]
    2.  
    1. ‘I saw him/that man/this man’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. chistu
    2. this.MSG
    1. /
    2.  
    1. chidu
    2. that.MSG
    1. /
    2.  
    1. *idu
    2.   him
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1. [–human]
    2.  
    1. ‘I saw this (thing)/that (thing)’

Based on this evidence, two semantic and syntactic types of third person pronouns can be identified. Type A (Table 2) corresponds to human referents and, by implication, presuppositional third person pronouns (11a). We assume that Type A is endowed with a Person feature, which in our account includes humanness by default.14 This assumption departs from Harley & Ritter’s (2002) account whereby the third person pronoun is not a true personal form. Type B third person pronouns refer to non-human entities (11b), therefore failing to convey the default presuppositionality entailments shown by 1st and 2nd pronouns and lacking Person (see also Anagnostopoulou 2003: 267–272; Richards 2008).

Table 2

Pronouns.

person humanness presuppositionality entailments Person
1 + + +
2 + + +
3 – Type A + + +
3 – Type B - - -

Proper names and singular kinship terms equally denote [+human/+definite] referents and are presupposed by the speech act participants. Therefore, they assimilate to third person DPs of the type A and are endowed with [+Person].

When specified [+human], in Calabrese1 other third person nominals (e.g. definite DPs, indefinite pronouns, bare universal quantifiers and demonstratives) display the same behaviour as personal pronouns. In such cases, they semantically and syntactically cluster with the first/second personal pronouns, i.e. type 3A in Table 2, and display DOM. By contrast, third person nominals of type B are indefinite, non-human or inanimate and non-presuppositional on the basis of their inherent features, such that DOM consistently fails to obtain with them. Given these assumptions, a strong correlation holds for Calabrese1 (12), i.e. if the direct object is [+Person] (13a), accusative is realised with DOM; if the direct object is [–Person] (13b), DOM is not given.

(12) + Person = + DOM
  Person = DOM
    1. (13)
    1. a.
    1. Mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. *(a)u
    2.    to.the.MSG
    1. previte.
    2. priest
    1. [+DOM, +Person]
    2.  
    1. ‘S/he killed the priest.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG.PRT
    1. (*a)u
    2.    to.the.MSG
    1. gattu.
    2. cat
    1. [–DOM, –Person]
    2.  
    1. ‘I saw the cat.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Ruppiu
    2. broke.1SG
    1. (*a)u
    2.    to.the.MSG
    1. tavulu.
    2. table.MSG
    1. ‘I broke the table.’

Theoretically, we therefore claim that, in order for Agree to occur, v is endowed with a strong unvalued D-feature, i.e. Person, semantically endowed with humanness. Direct object DPs with third person morphology can display two types of values, i.e. [± Person]. In order to derive (13a), the feature [+Person] needs to be probed by v in the course of the accusative valuation.

    1. (14)

As the direct object DP u previte ‘the priest’ is [+Person], it functions as a non-defective Goal for the head v to probe the Person feature. In order for the uCase feature to be valued through the D head on the [+Person] DP, the marker a must be inserted. Conversely, the Person specification is redundant for inanimate, generic, non-presuppositional DPs (Richards 2008: 14ff). We assume that they exhibit [–Person], i.e. the absence of the feature. Therefore, the DP acts as a defective Goal (cf. Roberts 2018: 118) and Person is left unvalued in the derivation. As a result, the absence of Person is spelt out as third person morphology, i.e. the default morphological exponence (cf. Benveniste 1966: 256), no marker is inserted and accusative is left unmarked. Nevertheless, if the DP lacks Person, it still behaves as a non-defective Goal for v to value Case. As a result, the DP’s Case is valued.

In sum, in Calabrese1 the accusative of [+Person] DPs is always licensed through DOM.15 In particular, the accusative is valued through Agree: v values its D-feature on the animate DPs which are endowed with [+Person] and the resulting structure, which is spelled out with the accusative marker, is a scattered configuration due to the projection of KP (Giorgi & Pianesi 1997; see also Kremers 2009). Person is left unvalued as the non-human and generic DPs do not display a Person feature. This leads to default third person morphological exponence and no DOM.

3 Interpreting Calabrese2

The patterns of accusative realisation of Calabrese2 exhibit peculiar properties, as the direct object is never differentially marked if the definite determiner occurs (16a). Conversely, if the definite determiner is absent, the accusative is marked by a when conveying a presuppositional reading (16b vs 16c):

    1. (16)
    1. a.
    1. Petru mazzau
    2. Petru killed.3SG
    1. (*a)u
    2.    the.MSG
    1. previte.
    2. priest
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1. [±presuppositional]
    2.  
    1. ‘Petru killed the priest.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Petru mazzau
    2. Petru killed.3SG
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. nu
    2. a.MSG
    1. previte
    2. priest
    1. chi
    2. that
    1. canuscia
    2. knew.1SG
    1. eu.
    2. I
    1. [+presuppositional]
    2.  
    1. ‘Petru killed a certain priest whom I knew.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. (*a)
    2.    to
    1. nu
    2. a.MSG
    1. previte.
    2. priest
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1.  
    2.  
    1. [–presuppositional]
    2.  
    1. ‘Petru killed a priest.’

Calabrese2 shows, therefore, the following pattern:

(17) + Person = ± DOM
  Person = DOM

In Calabrese2, as in Calabrese1, generic and inanimate DPs do not require DOM, whereas DPs conveying a presuppositional entailment require a on condition that the D head is not lexicalised by a definite determiner. DOM is also obligatory with indefinite DPs if bearing a presuppositional interpretation.

The point of variation between the two systems is represented by the cases in which definite DPs are headed by the definite determiner and interpreted as presuppositional: this configuration leads to the realisation of the marker a in Calabrese1 (18a) and to the sole presence of the definite determiner in Calabrese2 (18b):

    1. (18)
    1. a.
    1. Calabrese1
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. mazzau
    2. killed.3sg
    1. o
    2. to.the.MSG
    1. previte
    2. priest
    1. chi
    2. that
    1. canuscia
    2. knew.1SG
    1. eu.
    2. I
    1. ‘Petru killed the priest whom I knew.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Calabrese2
    1. Petru
    2. Petru
    1. mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. (*a)u
    2.    to.the.MSG
    1. previte
    2. priest
    1. chi
    2. that
    1. canuscia
    2. knew.1SG
    1. eu.
    2. I
    1. ‘Petru killed the priest whom I knew.’

In Calabrese2 the opposition between [±presuppositional] is morphologically blurred when the definite determiner is lexicalised. The accusative in Calabrese2 is expressed through three different configurations of the direct object DP, which is either marked with a (<AD), is headed by the definite determiner (DET), or has no mark (Ø), as summarised in Table 3.

Table 3

Distribution of DOM in in Calabrese2.

DPs DOM
personal pronouns +
singular kinship terms +
DPs headed by definite determiner
indefinite DPs +

The accusative valuation on the DPs endowed with [+Person] can only result in two types of configurations, either one showing the definite determiner or the other requiring a. Therefore, the definite determiner and the marker a are in complementary distribution. In order to explain this micro-variation in accusative Case marking, we propose that accusative DPs in Calabrese2 undergo the same licensing mechanism as in Calabrese1, but with the crucial difference that, when Agree takes place in Calabrese2, the lexicalisation of the D head through the definite determiner is the necessary and sufficient condition for v to value the uCase feature on the [±Person] DP. Arguably, the Agree operation between v and the DP in which the D-position is filled with the definite determiner is the default mechanism of accusative valuation. The Person feature is valued covertly in the case of [+Person] DPs and no differential marker is required (16a, 18b). The DP is able to mark the accusative with the sole presence of the definite determiner and the KP layer is not projected. Therefore, the definite determiner spells out the syncretic head of a syntactically definite accusative DP, as shown in (19):

    1. (19)

If in the [+Person] DPs the D° position is not filled by the definite determiner but is occupied by a pronoun or a noun (20a) or D° is empty (20b), accusative valuation and Person valuation result in the insertion of a.

    1. (20)
    1. Gioiosa Ionica
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Stamatina
    2. this morning
    1. vitti
    2. saw.3SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. mia
    2. me
    1. /
    2.  
    1. a
    2. to
    1. figghita
    2. son.your
    1. /
    2.  
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Maria
    2. Maria
    1. ‘This morning he saw you/your son/Maria’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. du
    2. two
    1. figghjoli
    2. kids
    1. ‘I saw a few kids’

The accusative is realised through a scattered DP structure where the marker a is merged in the head of KP (21). Finally, when the DP exhibits no Person, no marker a can be merged in the structure, resulting in the default third person morphology.

    1. (21)

In Calabrese2 the lexicalisation of the D head through the definite article is sufficient for Agree to check a bundle of features, including [+Person] and accusative Case. This property is related to the definite article’s ability to head a DP that acts as a non-defective Goal for Case valuation. At the same time, the definite article is also the marker of [+Person]. Therefore, its presence makes the insertion of a redundant. This pattern of accusative licensing is available to other Romance varieties, i.e. Sardinian, Corsican and Catalan. However, it is significant that such a mechanism of accusative valuation is found in southern Calabria, an area of pervasive and long contact between Greek and Latin/Romance. In the same varieties, the lexicalisation of D° also affects the realisation of dative. In order to mark dative, Calabrese2 displays the Greek-style genitive marker di only when D is filled with the definite article.16 For a detailed description and a structural account of the complementary distribution of the dative Case realisation in southern Calabria we refer to Ledgeway et al. (2016; forthcoming; see also Chilà 2017). In the next section we discuss the possibility that language contact determined the complementary distribution between the definite article and DOM.

4 Case-marking and the Greek-Romance contact in southern Calabria

Some Romance dialects of southern Calabria display indisputable structural influences from the Italo-Greek varieties spoken in the same area.17 In the domain of morphosyntax, a property exhibited by Italo-Greek which has been transferred into some surrounding Romance varieties concerns the requirement that D° be lexicalised with an expletive article in conjunction with proper names. Variation in the use of the article with proper names is the reflex of a deeper abstract property of nominals, namely Strong Person in Longobardi & Guardiano’s (2009) terms. This property requires D to be visible in order to license a referential interpretation of its associated noun (Longobardi 1994; 2008). Southern Italian dialects display this property as proper names overtly raise to D. Yet, such movement is blocked in the variety of San Luca (22a), which belongs to the Calabrese2 sub-set in our discussion, in the Romance dialects of Salento (22b), as well as in all Greek varieties of southern Italy (23), where the expletive definite article is used instead:

    1. (22)
    1. a.
    1. San Luca
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG.PRT
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. Petru
    2. Pietro
    1. nt’a
    2. in.the.FSG
    1. chiazza.
    2. square
    1. ‘I saw Pietro in the square.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Calimera, Salento
    1. Lu
    2. the.MSG
    1. Pietru
    2. Pietro
    1. ntise
    2. heard.3SG
    1. la
    2. the.FSG
    1. Maria.
    2. Maria
    1. ‘Pietro heard Maria.’
    1. (23)
    1. a.
    1. Corigliano, Salento
    1. I
    2. the
    1. Maria
    2. Maria
    1. ítela
    2. wanted.3SG
    1. na
    2. SBJV
    1. vorasi
    2. buy.SBJV.3SG
    1. na
    2. a.NSG
    1. spiti.
    2. house.NSG
    1. ‘Maria wanted to buy a house.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Bova, Calabria
    1. O
    2. the.MSG
    1. Petro
    2. Petro
    1. tragudai
    2. sings
    1. calà.
    2. well
    1. ‘Petro sings well.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Gallicianò, Calabria
    1. ecinde
    2. those.FPL
    1. micceddhe
    2. girls
    1. tu
    2. of.the.GEN.MSG
    1. Righíu
    2. Reggio.GEN
    1. ‘those girls from Reggio’

Such an evident contact feature between Greek and Romance has arguably played a crucial role in the patterns of case marking observed in Calabrese2. Italo-Greek determiners display cumulative overt exponence of features, as not only are they able to express gender and number, but they also convey Case as part of a portmanteau realisation (Ledgeway et al. in prep.). We can suppose that the tripartite inflectional Case system of Calabrian Greek (“Case System I” in Table 4) was transferred into Romance. Within the syntax of the Italo-Romance dialects, two possible scenarios in the Case marking arose, “Case System II” and “Case System III” in Table 4.

Table 4

Case markings in southern Calabria.

Calabrian Greek Calabrese
Case System I Case System II Case System III
(m & f definite determiners) nouns not raising to D nouns raising to D
NOM o, i DEF.DET Ø
ACC ton, tin a
DAT tu, tis di + DEF.DET
GEN di

“Case System II” occurs if nouns cannot move to the D area, as D° is filled with the definite determiner. The definite article lexicalises φ-features and Case at the same time, exactly as in Calabrian Greek. In this configuration, no further marker is needed to mark accusative. In “Case System III”, nominals lexicalise D° in order to satisfy referential requirements. In this configuration, no definite determiner can be lexicalised. Hence, accusative is marked by DOM.

Given this account of the case systems of Greek and Romance varieties of southern Calabria, the point of interference from contact becomes evident: in both Case System I and Case System II the mechanism of structural accusative valuation requires the merger of a DP whose head syncretically realises φ- and Case features. Given that Case System II is found in dialects spoken in the past by bilingual Romance/Greek speakers, we can suppose that it emerged as an innovative configuration, representing a hybrid structure in which the realisation of accusative and dative cases results from the combinations of (morpho)syntactic properties of Calabrian Greek, the competing language. The emergence of such forms is extremely revealing for the debate on the notion of transfer in the context of language contact and change. According to Aboh (2015), interfaces play the relevant role in the selection and recombination of linguistic features. In particular, the syntax-discourse/semantics interface within the noun phrase represents a vulnerable area of the grammar for transfer to happen (Aboh 2015: 171ff). In the competing grammars of southern Calabria, features such as Definiteness, Person and Specificity/Presuppositionality, visible at the syntax-discourse/semantics interface, have determined two different mechanisms, which can be defined as pattern transmission18 (24) and feature transmission (25) (Aboh 2015: 172). In the varieties of southern Calabria pattern transmission is witnessed in Case System II. Such hybrid Case configurations are the result of selecting and retaining Person feature (Fx) as well as Definiteness features (Fx) from Greek (Lx), the historically competing language, and adopting their semantics and licensing properties:

(24) Aboh (2015: 172)
  Fx [Function = Lx; Syntax = Lx] -> Pattern transmission

The Romance Case System III may be interpreted as the result of feature transmission (25), in that some Italo-Romance dialects of southern Calabria selected Definiteness and Person on the basis of their function in Greek, while leaving their licensing mechanism to be determined by Romance syntax:

(25) Aboh (2015: 172)
  Fx [Function = Lx; Syntax = … ] -> Feature transmission

Bilingual speakers in the villages of southern Calabria were exposed to at least two grammars and can therefore be identified as the locus of linguistic change19 (cf. Roeper 1999; Lightfoot 2006). Nowadays, the Greek features are mostly kept in the language of the older generation. The relevant evidence is represented by the accusative marking of surnames and nicknames that have recently entered the lexical repertoire of these varieties. Younger speakers of the dialect of San Luca allow DOM to mark them as accusative (i.e. Case System III; 26a,b), whereas speakers of earlier generations use the definite determiner and exclude DOM (i.e. Case System II; 26c, d):

    1. (26)
    1. San Luca
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Mama
    2. mom
    1. jiu
    2. went.3SG.PRT
    1. a’
    2. the.FSG
    1. Merica
    2. America
    1. e
    2. and
    1. vitti
    2. saw.3SG.PRT
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Trump
    2. Trump
    1. ‘Mom went to the States and saw Trump.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. ‘Nto
    2. in.the.MSG
    1. programma
    2. show.MSG
    1. minaru
    2. beat.up.3PL.PRT
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Fedez.
    2. Fedez
    1. ‘During the show they’ve beaten up Fedez.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Mama
    2. mom
    1. jiu
    2. went.3SG.PRT
    1. a’
    2. the.FSG
    1. Merica
    2. America
    1. e
    2. and
    1. vitti
    2. saw.3SG.PRT
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. Trump.
    2. Trump
    1. ‘Mom went to the States and saw Trump.’
    1.  
    1. d.
    1. ‘Nto
    2. in.the.MSG
    1. programma
    2. show.MSG
    1. minaru
    2. beat.up.3PL.PRT
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. Fedez.
    2. Fedez
    1. ‘During the show, they’ve beaten up Fedez.’

Such younger speakers act as the agents of the linguistic change in the Case system, as they received the inputs necessary to complete the pattern transmission (25) in their emerging grammar. Even though Greek later ceased to be spoken, the language transmission was successful and resulted in the observed configuration of accusative and dative.

4.1 A parameter hierarchy of accusative Case checking

The Case marking systems of southern Calabrian varieties display fine-grained morphosyntactic variation. To understand it, we adopt a theory that departs from the notion of Universal Grammar as a defined set of parametrised options (Borer 1984), but, rather, takes parameters to be mutually dependent: the ones which are highly local and related to the surface-oriented variation (microparameters) cluster together to form parameters of greater import (macroparameters). More specifically, macroparameters are composed of aggregates of microparameters acting in unison.20

The hierarchy in (27) is conceived as a typological classification based on the valuation of a Case feature (ACC) and [+Person] ([+PER]). If the DP is not able to have [+PER] valued through the D head, the marker a must be inserted in K°. The relevant varieties of the hierarchy show different patterns of accusative marking depending on whether D° is lexicalised by a nominal element and what the nature of such an element is. The Person-licensing property of the DP is given even if D° is empty.21 Given the evidence discussed above, when v cannot value Person as the DP is [–PER] (i.e. generic or inanimate), the spelt-out structure expresses a default third person morphology. The classification in (27) does not cover this aspect of variation, as it only concerns [+PER] DPs.

In Calabrese1 (28a,b), as well as in some other Romance varieties such as spoken Catalan (28c), all [+PER] DPs require the merger of the marker a in K°. Standard modern Greek (29a,b) and Italo-Greek (29c,d) never differentially mark the direct object, since [+PER] DPs have their D feature valued by vACC always through D°, whether it is lexicalised or empty. Some other Romance varieties (e.g. standard Italian, French) do not value the accusative of [+PER] DPs through a scattered configuration including the KP layer. Yet, in our classification they cannot cluster with standard Modern Greek and Italo-Greek, as in French and standard Italian N-to-D movement is a viable option so that D° is also lexicalised by proper names. Lexicalisation of D° through the definite article is the sufficient condition for Calabrese2 (30a), Sardinian2 (30b), Corsican (30c) and Catalan (30d) to value [+PER] on the accusative DP. Some varieties of Sardinian (Sardinian1 (31)) insert the marker a in K° if D° is occupied by a definite article (31a), whereas if D° is empty (31b) [+PER] is valued without projecting the KP layer. In the Abruzzese dialect of Arielli ((32); see fn.14), the lexicalisation of the D head is able to value [+PER] only when it hosts third person nominals, i.e. proper names and pronouns.

    1. (27)
    1. (28)
    1. Embrisi
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. o
    2. to.the.MSG
    1. previte.
    2. priest
    1. ‘He killed the priest.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Vitti
    2. saw.1SG
    1. a
    2. to
    1. du
    2. two
    1. omini.
    2. men
    1. ‘I saw two men.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Spoken Central Catalan22 (Hualde 1992: 241)
    1. Les
    2. the
    1. monges
    2. nuns
    1. no
    2. not
    1. estimen
    2. like.3PL
    1. a/ana
    2. to
    1. les
    2. the
    1. nenes.
    2. girls
    1. ‘The nuns do not like the girls.’
    1. (29)
    1. Standard Modern Greek
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Ida
    2. see.PST.PRF.1SG
    1. ta
    2. the.ACC.NPL
    1. pediá.
    2. kids
    1. ‘I saw the kids.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Diávasa
    2. read.PST.PRF.1SG
    1. lighes
    2. few.ACC.FPL
    1. grammés.
    2. lines.ACC.FPL
    1. ‘I have read a few lines.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Greco, Gallicianò
    1. O
    2. the.MSG
    1. Petro
    2. Petro
    1. aggue
    2. hear.PST.PRF.3SG
    1. tin
    2. the.ACC.FSG
    1. Maria.
    2. Maria
    1. ‘Petro heard Maria.’
    1.  
    1. d.
    1. Greco, Bova Marina
    1. Ivra
    2. see.PST.PRF.1SG
    1. dio
    2. two
    1. magna
    2. beautiful.ACC.NPL
    1. sciddía.
    2. dogs.ACC.NPL
    1. ‘I saw two beautiful dogs.’
    1. (30)
    1. a.
    1. San Luca
    1. Mazzau
    2. killed.3SG
    1. u
    2. the.MSG
    1. previte
    2. priest
    1. i
    2. of
    1. Messina.
    2. Messina
    1. ‘He killed the priest of Messina.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Sardinian2 (Jones 1995: 39)23
    1. Appu
    2. have.1SG
    1. vistu
    2. seen
    1. su
    2. the.MSG
    1. mere
    2. boss
    1. /
    2. /
    1. su
    2. the.MSG
    1. dottore
    2. doctor
    1. /
    2. /
    1. su
    2. the.MSG
    1. re.
    2. king
    1. ‘I have seen the boss/ the doctor / the king.’
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Corsican (Neuburger & Stark 2014: 366)
    1. Cunnusciti
    2. know.2PL
    1. (*à)
    2.    to
    1. U
    2. the.MSG
    1. Scupatu?
    2. Scupatu
    1. ‘Do you know the Scupatu?’
    1.  
    1. d.
    1. Catalan (Escandell-Vidal 2009: 838)
    1. Només
    2. only
    1. va
    2. AUX.PST.3SG
    1. invitar
    2. invite
    1. la
    2. the.FSG
    1. Maria
    2. Maria
    1. i
    2. and
    1. *(a)
    2.    to
    1. ell.
    2. him
    1. ‘S/he only invited Maria and him.’
    1. (31)
    1. Sardinian1 (Jones 1995: 38–39)
    1. Appu
    2. have.1SG
    1. vistu
    2. seen
    1. a
    2. to
    1. su
    2. the.MSG
    1. dottore
    2. doctor
    1. de
    2. of
    1. Rosaria.
    2. Rosaria
    1. ‘I saw Rosaria’s doctor.’
    1. (32)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. *so
    2.   I.am
    1. vistə
    2. seen
    1. a
    2. to
    1. Marije
    2. Marije
    1. /
    2. /
    1. a
    2. to
    1. jisse
    2. them
    1. /
    2. /
    1. a
    2. to
    1. quille
    2. them
    1.    ‘I saw Marije / them / them’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. so
    2. I.am
    1. vistə
    2. seen
    1. a
    2. to
    1. tte
    2. you
    1. ‘I saw you’

Other than a powerful descriptive tool, this hierarchy models a theoretical interpretation of the role of animacy and definiteness in differential case-marking. Thus, it takes into account more than just morphological exponence. The splits are based on the fine-grained structure of the DP and the mechanism of D-feature valuation. In this way, DOM is interpreted as an option of the syntactic phenomenon of Case licensing and is linked to the syntactic conditioning factors.

5 Conclusions

In the Romance dialects of southern Calabria, DOM is required to convey a presuppositional interpretation on the direct object which, in order to be marked as accusative, triggers the appearance of the marker a. This generalisation is not valid for a subgroup of varieties (Calabrese2) which exhibit a complementary distribution between the marker a and the definite determiner. The lexicalisation of the latter rules out the realisation of DOM, irrespective of the presuppositional entailments. We have shown that this can be accounted for as a reflex of the valuation process of Case and Person features. More specifically, D bears a [+Person] feature to be valued by v, which in turn values an accusative Case feature on the DP. Once this feature bundle is checked through Agree, the accusative is licensed without the insertion of a. Independent evidence from Sardinian, Corsican and Catalan clearly suggests that this configuration is a parametric option available across Romance. As for the trigger of such a configuration in southern Calabria, the long-standing Romance-Greek contact has played a highly relevant role. The structural parallel with the properties of D exhibited by the surrounding Greek varieties is striking. The definite determiner in Calabrian Greek is the morphosyntactic outcome of a bundle of features, including Case, such that no accusative marker surfaces. The Romance dialects of southern Calabria have integrated this property of Italo-Greek syntax, filtering the grammar of accusative valuation through the Romance case licensing strategies. The result is therefore an accommodation of a Greek syntactic pattern within Romance syntax, ultimately leading to the emergence of hybrid patterns. In this respect, accusative marking follows a trend that the present-day Romance varieties of the extreme south of Italy exhibit through several morphosyntactic aspects: accusative is realised through differential marking in accordance with specific factors, among which we singled out the checking of a [+Person] feature. Finally, the variation in patterns of case marking in such a long-standing contact area contributes to the debate on the emergence and structural configurations of hybrid systems.

Abbreviations

1 = first person, 2 = second person, 3 = third person, ACC = accusative, ART = article, AUX = auxiliary, COND = conditional, DAT = dative, DEF = definite, DEM = demonstrative, DET = determiner, F = feminine, GEN = genitive, GER = gerund, IND = indicative, INF = infinitive, M = masculine, N = neuter, NOM = nominative, OBJ = object, OBL = oblique, PER = person, PL = plural, PRF = perfect, PRS = present, PST = past, PTCP = participle, SBJV = subjunctive, SG = singular

Notes

  1. DOM is attested in standard Italian as well (Berruto 1985; Benincà 1988; Berretta 1989; Zamboni 1991; Lorenzetti 2002). [^]
  2. In the Gallo-Romance dialects of Sicily DOM is signaled by da < DE + AB (Rohlfs 1969: 8, 15; Manzini & Savoia 2005, II: 502); in the central Italian dialects by ma/me < IN MEDIO (AD) (Rolhfs 1969: 15; Berizzi 2013). The markers a, da, ma/me also signal dative in these same varieties (Rohlfs 1971: 333–335). [^]
  3. See, among others, Rohlfs (1969: §632; 1971); Jones (1995); Sornicola (1997a; b; 2011); Ledgeway (2000: Chapter 2); Manzini & Savoia (2005, II: 515ff); Iemmolo (2010); Guardiano (2014); Andriani (2015). [^]
  4. The relevant data have been collected through interviews with native speakers of Italo-Romance varieties of the extreme south of Calabria from April to July 2016. The fieldwork investigations fall within the scope of the Research Project “Fading voices in southern Italy: investigating language contact in Magna Graecia”, carried out at the University of Cambridge. Additional information can be found on the project’s website: www.greekromanceproject.wordpress.com. [^]
  5. Thanks to the reference suggested by an anonymous reviewer, we also include the accusative marking system of Agnana Calabra as fully consistent with Calabrese1 (Bentley et al. 2015: 141–142). Agnana Calabra is a village of the north-eastern side of the Aspromonte massif. [^]
  6. At the time of the investigations (April–July 2016), the Italo-Greek variety of Calabria was still spoken by a small number of predominantly elderly people in five villages: Bova (Marina), Chorío di Roghudi, Condofuri (Marina), Gallicianò, Roghudi (Nuovo). A variety of Italo-Greek is also spoken in Salento, in seven villages south of Lecce (for further details, see Schifano & Silvestri 2017). [^]
  7. Most varieties of the extreme south of Calabria display aphaeresized forms of the definite articles:
      1. (i)
      1. Embrisi
      1. U
      2. the.MSG
      1. previte,
      2. priest
      1. a
      2. the.FSG
      1. cotrara
      2. girl
      1. e
      2. and
      1. i
      2. the.FSG
      1. monachi
      2. nuns
      1. chiamaru
      2. called.3SG.PRT
      1. ajeri.
      2. yesterday
      1. ‘Yesterday the priest/the girl/the nuns called up.’
    Non-aphaeresized forms of the definite article are employed if the following word begins with a vowel:
      1. (ii)
      1. Embrisi
      1. l’
      2. the.MSG
      1. occhiu,
      2. eye
      1. l’
      2. the.FSG
      1. acqua,
      2. water
      1. l’
      2. the.FPL
      1. ossa,
      2. bones
      1. l’
      2. the.MPL
      1. atri
      2. others
      1. ‘the eye, the water, the bones, the others’
    When the marker a co-occurs with the aphaeresized forms of the definite determiners, vocalic coalescence obtains, namely [a] + [a] = [aː], [a] + [ʊ] = [o], [a] + [ɪ] = [e]. The lengthening of /a/ is often not detectable, so that the quantitative difference between the bear FSG definite determiner and the articulated form is blurred. In order to graphically disambiguate the FSG definite determiner and the articulated form from the marker a, we represent the FSG definite determiner as ‘a, the articulated form as â and the marker as a. The marker a co-occurring with the non-aphaeresized forms of the definite determiners triggers the lengthening of the /l/:
      1. (iii)
      1. Embrisi
      1. Vitti
      2. saw.1SG
      1. a-
      2. to
      1. ll’
      2. the.MPL
      1. atri
      2. others
      1. ajeri.
      2. yesterday
      1. ‘I saw the others yesterday.’
    [^]
  8. The dialects of Gioiosa Ionica and San Luca too exhibit the aphaeresized forms (see fn.5):
      1. (i)
      1. Gioiosa Ionica
      1. U
      2. the.MSG
      1. cani,
      2. dog
      1. a
      2. the.FSG
      1. crapa
      2. sheep
      1. e
      2. and
      1. i
      2. the.M.FPL
      1. gatti
      2. cats
      1. mi
      2. me.OBJ=
      1. muzzicaru.
      2. bite.3SG.PRT
      1. ‘The dog, the sheep and the cats bit me.’
      1. (ii)
      1. San Luca
      1. U
      2. the.MSG
      1. previti,
      2. priest
      1. a
      2. the.FSG
      1. mastra
      2. seamstress
      1. e
      2. and
      1. i
      2. the
      1. cotrari
      2. kids
      1. mi
      2. me.OBJ=
      1. salutaru.
      2. wave.3SG.PRT
      1. ‘The priest, the seamstress and the kids have waved at me.’
    Non-aphaeresized forms of the definite articles occur when the following word begins with a vowel:
      1. (iii)
      1. Gioiosa Ionica
      1. Mi
      2. me=
      1. cicasti
      2. blinded.2SG.PRT
      1. l’
      2. the.MSG
      1. occhiu.
      2. eye
      1. ‘You have blinded me (lit.= you have blinded my eye).’
      1. (iv)
      1. San Luca
      1. Vippi
      2. drank.1SG.PRT
      1. l’
      2. the.FSG
      1. acqua
      2. water
      1. fridda.
      2. cold.FSG
      1. ‘I have drunk some cold water.’
      1. (v)
      1. San Luca
      1. Vitti
      2. saw.1SG.PRT
      1. l’
      2. the.MSG
      1. Armandu
      2. Armando
      1. /
      2. /
      1. la
      2. the.FSG
      1. Ida
      2. Ida
      1. /
      2. /
      1. l’
      2. the.MPL
      1. atri.
      2. others
      1. ‘I saw Armando/Ida/the others.’
    Also, non-aphaeresized forms of the definite article surface when the preposition a (<AD), in its locative function, precedes the definite DPs. In these cases, a triggers the lengthening of /l/ in Gioiosa Ionica (vi) whereas in San Luca /l/ undergoes also palatalisation (vii).
      1. (vi)
      1. Gioiosa Ionica
      1. Vaju
      2. go.1SG.PRS
      1. a-lla
      2. to-the.FSG
      1. casa
      2. house
      1. i
      2. of
      1. tata
      2. dad
      1. /
      2. /
      1. a- llu
      2. to-the.MSG
      1. mari.
      2. sea
      1. ‘I am going to my dad’s/to the seaside.’
      1. (vii)
      1. San Luca
      1. Staiu
      2. stand.1SG.PRS
      1. jendu
      2. do.GER
      1. a-glia
      2. to-the.FSG
      1. casa
      2. house
      1. d’a Maria
      2. of.the.FSG Maria
      1. /
      2. /
      1. a-gliu
      2. to-the.MSG
      1. mari.
      2. sea
      1. ‘I am going to Maria’s/to the seaside.’
    [^]
  9. The evidence brought up by Calabrese2 finds a striking parallel in patterns of DOM displayed in Sardinian (Jones 1993: 65–76; 1995) and Corsican (Neuburger & Stark 2014). [^]
  10. The phonetic changes triggered by /a/ (<AD), as described in fn.8, help prove that in San Luca an underlying AD as a mark of the direct object is ruled out:
      1. (i)
      1. San Luca
      1. Vitti
      2. saw.1SG.PRT
      1. *a-glia
      2.   to-the.FSG
      1. Maria
      2. Maria
      1. /
      2.  
      1. *a-gliu
      2.   to-the.MSG
      1. Petru.
      2. Petru
      1. ‘I saw Maria/Pietro.’
    [^]
  11. The same generalisation cannot be put forward for Calabrese2 as a whole, as no N-to-D movement is attested in the dialects of San Luca and Gioiosa Ionica. [^]
  12. Indexicals are prototypically presuppositional in that they determine a referent only in conjunction with elements of the utterance context, i.e. the speech act presupposes a speaker and an addressee (Abbott 2010: 180). For implicational relations between Person and animacy see Adger & Harbour (2007); for Person and definiteness/specificity see Richards (2004; 2008). [^]
  13. Some Sardinian varieties avail themselves of parallel DOM patterns (Jones 1995: 48–61). In (26) we record two different patterns of Sardinian. [^]
  14. However, in Ariellese (a variety of Abruzzese) the accusative of the animate third person pronouns is not marked with a (D’Alessandro 2012; see also §4.1). [^]
  15. In southern Italian dialects (Ledgeway 2000: 37; Manzini & Savoia 2005, II: 502ff), as well as in Spanish (Jaeggli 1982; Torrego 1998; Leonetti 2008) and in Romanian (Dobrovie-Sorin 1990), the specific interpretation of internal arguments often requires clitic doubling. This correlation, which is valid for the set of data discussed in this paper as well, proves stronger when the object is a tonic pronoun (e.g. Neapolitan; Sornicola 1997a; b; Ledgeway 2009: 831ff). [^]
      1. (i)
      1. Gioiosa Ionica (Ledgeway et al. forthcoming)
      1. Ajeri
      2. yesterday
      1. nci
      2. DAT.3=
      1. telefonau
      2. I.phoned
      1. a
      2. to
      1. nu
      2. a.MSG
      1. previte.
      2. priest
      1. ‘Yesterday I phoned a priest.’
      1. (ii)
      1. (Nci)
      2. DAT.3=
      1. u
      2. it=
      1. dissi
      2. I.told
      1. d-u
      2. of-the.MSG
      1. previte…
      2. priest
      1. ‘I told the priest…’
    [^]
  16. Influence in the opposite direction is also detectable, especially among younger members of the speech community (Schifano & Silvestri 2017; Ledgeway et al. 2017; forthcoming). [^]
  17. Cf. the mechanism of PAT(tern) transmission in Matras & Sakel (2004; 2007). [^]
  18. In the Aspromonte area (Bovesìa), Greek was still spoken extensively alongside the Romance varieties at the end of the 18th century (Martino 1980: 311; Piromalli 1996: 419–420; see also Rohlfs 1972; Fanciullo 2005–2006). The same chronology cannot be maintained for the area where Calabrese2 is spoken (Locride area). From the scarce sources available, one can speculate that there Greek ceased to be used by Greek-Romance bilingual speakers by the end of the 17th century (Alessandro De Angelis p.c.). [^]
  19. See publications by “Rethinking Comparative Syntax” (ReCoS) research group based in Cambridge on the website: www.recos-dtal.mml.cam.ac.uk/Publications. [^]
  20. We assume that in the relevant varieties of the hierarchy, with the exception of standard Modern Greek, D° is empty also in DPs introduced by demonstratives (see 21) which occupy the SpecDP position (Brugè 1996; Giusti 1997; Guardiano 2009 a.o.). The empirical evidence we discuss here proves that, for the same given variety, DPs introduced by demonstratives show the same patterns of accusative realisation as the DPs introduced by definite articles:
      1. (i)
      1. Scido
      1. Vitti
      2. saw.1SG
      1. *(a)
      2.    to
      1. chiru
      2. that.MSG
      1. previte.
      2. priest
      1. ‘I saw that priest.’
      1. (ii)
      1. Vitti
      2. saw.1SG
      1. (*a)
      2.    to
      1. chiru
      2. that.MSG
      1. cani.
      2. dog
      1. ‘I saw that dog.’
      1. (iii)
      1. Vitti
      2. saw.1SG
      1. (*a)
      2.    to
      1. chira
      2. that.FSG
      1. machina.
      2. car
      1. ‘I saw that car.’
    One can argue that the Spec-Head relation between the demonstrative and D° allows the demonstrative to inherit the Person-licensing property of D°. [^]
  21. We adopt this label following Escandell-Vidal (2009: 836) who refers to spoken Central Catalan. [^]
  22. The Sardinian varieties investigated by Jones (1993; 1995) are spoken in the Bitti-Lula area (Nuoro; Jones 1995: 37). [^]

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to Annamaria Chilà and Alessandro De Angelis for their comments and suggestions on an earlier version of this manuscript. We are also thankful to the audience of the Humboldt Universität Berlin (‘Research Seminar for Research Unit for Experimental Syntax and Heritage Languages’) and of the University of Cambridge (‘NEREUS 2016: Referential Properties in the Romance DP in the Context of Multilingualism’), as well as to three anonymous reviewers, for their extremely constructive feedback. All errors remain our responsibility.

Funding Information

This work is part of the Research Project RPG-2015-283 ‘Fading voices in southern Italy: investigating language contact in Magna Graecia’, funded by the Leverhulme Trust and held at the Faculty of Modern and Medieval Languages of the University of Cambridge (www.greekromanceproject.wordpress.com).

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Author Contributions

The entire article is the result of joint work of the three authors in all respects. For the administrative purposes of the Italian academia only, Norma Schifano takes responsibility for section 1, Giuseppina Silvestri for sections 2 and 3 and Adam Ledgeway for sections 4 and 5.

References

Abbott, Barbara. 2006. Where have some of the presuppositions gone? In Betty Birner & Gregory Ward (eds.), Drawing the boundaries of meaning: Neo-Gricean studies in pragmatics and semantics in honor of Laurence R. Horn, 1–20. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.80.02abb

Abbott, Barbara. 2010. Reference. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aboh, Enoch. 2015. The Emergence of hybrid grammars. Language contact and change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139024167

Adger, David & Daniel Harbour. 2007. The syntax and syncretisms of the Person Case Constraint. Syntax 10. 2–37. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9612.2007.00095.x

Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 21(3). 435–483. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024109008573

Anagnostopoulou, Elena. 2003. The syntax of ditransitives. Evidence from clitics. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Andriani, Luigi. 2015. Semantic and syntactic properties of the prepositional accusative in Barese. Linguistica Atlantica 34(2). 61–78.

Bárány, András. 2018. Person, case, and agreement. The morphosyntax of inverse agreement and global case splits. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198804185.001.0001

Benincà, Paola. 1988. L’ordine degli elementi della frase e le costruzioni marcate. Costruzionicon ordine marcato degli elementi. In Lorenzo Renzi (ed.), Grande grammatica italiana diconsultazione, 129–143. Bologna: Il Mulino.

Bentley, Delia, Francesco Ciconte & Silvio Cruschina. 2015. Existentials and locatives in Romance dialects of Italy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198745266.001.0001

Benveniste, Émile. 1966. Problèmes de linguistique générale. Paris: Gallimard.

Berizzi, Mariachiara. 2013. The preposition ma in the central Italian dialects. Quaderni di lavoro ASIt 15. 1–21. http://asit.maldura.unipd.it/documenti/ql15/ASIt15_berizzi.pdf.

Berretta, Monica. 1989. Sulla presenza dell’accusativo preposizionale nell’italiano settentrionale: Note tipologiche. Vox Romanica 48. 13–37.

Berruto, Gaetano. 1985. Dislocazione a sinistra e grammatica dell’italiano parlato. In Annalisa Franchi De Bellis & Leonardo M. Savoia (eds.), Sintassi e morfologia della lingua italiana d’uso. Teorie e applicazioni descrittive, Atti Società linguistica italiana, 59–82. Roma: Bulzoni.

Borer, Hagit. 1984. Parametric syntax. Case studies in Semitic and Romance languages. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Bossong, Georg. 1982. Der präpositionale Akkusativ im Sardischen. In Otto Winkelmann & Maria Braisch (eds.), Festschrift für Johannes Hubschmid zum 65. Geburtstag, 579–599. Bern: Francke.

Bossong, Georg. 1985. Empirische Universalienforschung: Differentielle Objektmarkierung in den neuiranischen Sprachen. Tübingen: Narr.

Brugè, Laura. 1996. Demonstrative movement in Spanish: A comparative approach. University of Venice Working Papers in Linguistics 6. 1–53.

Campos, Héctor. 1999. Transitividad e intranstividad. In Ignacio Bosque & Violeta Demonte (eds.) Gramática descriptiva de la lengua española, 1519–1574. Madrid: Espasa-Calpe.

Carnie, Andrew. 2005. Some remarks on markedness hierarchies: A reply to Aissen 1999 and 2003. Coyote Papers 14. 37–50.

Cennamo, Michela. 2003. (In)transitivity and object marking: Some current issues. Inå Giuliana Fiorentino (ed.), Romance objects. Transitivity in Romance languages, 49–104. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110919837.49

Chilà, Aannamaria. 2017. Il sincretismo genitivo-dativo nella varietà reggina di S. Luca. L’Italia Dialettale 78. 57–71.

Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Roger Martin, David Michaels & Juan Uriagereka (eds.), Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, 89–155. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Michael Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

D’Alessandro, Roberta. 2012. Merging probes. A typology of person splits and person-driven differential object marking. lingbuzz/001771.

De Angelis, Alessandro. In press. Articolo espletivo e marcatura differenziale dell’oggetto nel dialetto reggino di San Luca. L’Italia Dialettale 80.

de Swart, Peter & Helen de Hoop. 2007. Semantic Aspects of Differential Object Marking. In Estela Puig-Waldmüller (ed.), Proceedings of SuB11. Barcelona, 568–581. Barcelona: Universitat Pompeu Fabra Press.

Diesing, Molly. 1992. Indefinites. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Dixon, Robert. 1979. Ergativity. Language 55. 59–138. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.2307/412519

Dobrovie-Sorin, Carmen. 1990. Clitic doubling, wh-movement, and quantification in Romanian. Linguistic Inquiry 21. 351–398.

Dragomirescu, Adina & Alex Nicolae. 2016. Case. In Martin Maiden & Adam Ledgeway (eds.), The Oxford guide to the Romance languages, 911–923. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199677108.003.0056

Dryer, Matthew. 1996. Focus, Pragmatic Presupposition, and Activated Propositions. Journal of Pragmatics 26. 475–523. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(95)00059-3

Enç, Müvert. 1991. The semantics of specificity. Linguistic Inquiry 22(1). 1–25.

Escandell-Vidal, M. Victoria. 2009. Differential object marking and topicality: The case of Balearic Catalan. Studies in Language 33(4). 832–884. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sl.33.4.02esc

Fanciullo, Franco. 2005–2006. Ancora di latino e greco in Calabria. L’Italia Dialettale 66–67. 49–68.

García García, Marco. 2005. Differential object marking and informativeness. In Klaus von Heusinger, Georg Kaiser & Elisabeth Stark (eds.), Proceedings of the workshop ‘Specificity and the evolution/emergence of nominal determination systems in Romance’, 17–32. Universität Konstanz: Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft.

Gianollo, Chiara & Giuseppina Silvestri. In press. Key topics in semantics: Presupposition, anaphora, (in)definite nominal phrases, deixis, tense and aspect, negation. In Adam Ledgeway & Martin Maiden (eds.), Cambridge handbook of Romance linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Giorgi, Alessandra & Fabio Pianesi. 1997. Tense and aspect: From semantics to morphosyntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Giusti, Giuliana. 1997. The categorial status of determiners. In Liliane Haegeman (ed.), The new comparative syntax. Harlow: Addison Wesley Longman.

Guardiano, Cristina. 2009. The syntax of demonstratives. A parametric analysis. Paper presented at the Colloquium on Generative Grammar 19, April 1–3 2009, Vitoria-Gasteiz.

Guardiano, Cristina. 2014. Fenomeni di contatto sintattico in Italia meridionale? Alcune note comparative. Quaderni di lavoro ASIt 18. 73–102. http://asit.maldura.unipd.it/documenti/ql11/6_guardiano.pdf.

Harley, Heidi & Elizabeth Ritter. 2002. Person and number in pronouns. A feature-geometric analysis. Language 78(3). 482–526. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2002.0158

Hopper, Paul & Sandra Thompson. 1980. Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language 56. 251–299. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1980.0017

Hopper, Paul & Sandra Thompson. 1982. Studies in transitivity. New York: Academic Press.

Hualde, José Ignacio. 1992. Catalan. London: Routledge.

Iemmolo, Giorgio. 2010. Topicality and differential object marking: Evidence from Romance and beyond. Studies in Language 34(2). 239–272. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sl.34.2.01iem

Jaeggli, Osvaldo. 1982. Topics in Romance syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.

Jäger, Gerhard. 1995. Topics in dynamic semantics. Berlin: Humboldt Universität Berlin dissertation.

Jones, Michael. 1993. Sardinian syntax. London: Routledge.

Jones, Michael. 1995. The prepositional accusative in Sardinian: Its distribution and syntactic repercussions. In John Charles Smith & Martin Maiden (eds.), Linguistic theory and the Romance languages, 37–75. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.122.03jon

Kremers, Joost. 2009. Recursive linearization. The Linguistic Review 26. 135–166. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.2009.004

La Fauci, Nunzio. 1997. Per una teoria grammaticale del mutamento morfosintattico. Dal latino verso il romanzo. Pisa: Edizioni ETS.

Larrivée, Pierre. 2014. The syntax of pragmatics: the case of presuppositional negatives. Syntaxe et sémantique 15(1). 115–137. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3917/ss.015.0115

Lazard, Gilbert. 1984. Actance variations and categories of the object. In Frans Plank (ed.), Objects: Towards a theory of grammatical relations, 267–292. London: Academic Press.

Ledgeway, Adam. 2000. A comparative syntax of the dialects of southern Italy: A minimalist approach. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

Ledgeway, Adam. 2009. Grammatica diacronica del napoletano. Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9783484971288

Ledgeway, Adam. 2018. Parametric variation in DOM in Italo-Romance. Paper presented at the workshop ‘Differential object marking in Romance: Towards microvatiation’, 9–10 November 2018, Paris: Institut National des Langues et Civilisations Orientales.

Ledgeway, Adam, Norma Schifano & Giuseppina Silvestri. 2016. Investigating the properties of D in the varieties of the extreme south of Italy: Greek-style Genitive and Differential Object Marking. Paper presented at ‘NEREUS 2016: Referential properties in the Romance DP in the context of multilingualism’, 2–3 December 2016, University of Cambridge.

Ledgeway, Adam, Norma Schifano & Giuseppina Silvestri. 2017. Greek and Romance in Contact: When finite becomes non-finite. Paper presented at the workshop ‘Language variation and change in contact situations’, 5 September 2017, Stellenbosch University.

Ledgeway, Adam, Norma Schifano & Giuseppina Silvestri. Forthcoming. Micro-variation in dative marking in the Romance and Greek varieties of southern Italy. In Anna Pineda & Jaume Mateu (eds.), Dative structures and beyond. Berlin: Language Science Press.

Ledgeway, Adam, Norma Schifano & Giuseppina Silvestri. In preparation. Greek and Romance in southern Italy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Leonetti, Manuel. 2008. Specificity in clitic doubling and in differential object marking. Probus 20(1). 33–66. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/PROBUS.2008.002

Leonetti, Manuel. 2012. Indefiniteness and specificity. In José Ignacio Hualde, Antxon Olarrea & Erin O’Rourke (eds.), The handbook of Hispanic linguistics, 285–305. London: Blackwell. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1002/9781118228098.ch15

Lightfoot, David. 2006. How new languages emerge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616204

Longobardi, Giuseppe. 1994. Reference and proper names. Linguistic Inquiry 25. 609–665.

Longobardi, Giuseppe. 2008. Reference to individuals, person, and the variety of mapping parameters. In Alex Klinge & Høeg Müller (eds.), Essays on nominal determination: From morphology to discourse management, 189–221. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.99.11lon

Longobardi, Giuseppe & Cristina Guardiano. 2009. Evidence for syntax as a signal of historical relatedness. Lingua 119. 1679–1706. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2008.09.012

Longobardi, Giuseppe, Cristina Guardiano, Giuseppina Silvestri, Alessio Boattini & Andrea Ceolin. 2013. Toward a syntactic phylogeny of modern Indo-European languages. Journal of Historical Linguistics 3(1). 122–152. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/jhl.3-1.07lon.additional

Lorenzetti, Luca. 2002. L’italiano contemporaneo. Rome: Carocci.

Lyons, Christopher. 1999. Definiteness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511605789

Manzini, Maria Rita & Leonardo M. Savoia. 2005. I dialetti italiani e romanci. Morfosintassi generativa. Alessandria: Edizioni dell’Orso.

Martino, Paolo. 1980. L’isola grecanica dell’Aspromonte. Aspetti sociolinguistici. In Federico Albano Leoni (ed.), I dialetti e le lingue delle minoranze di fronte all’italiano, 305–341. Roma: Bulzoni.

Matras, Yaron & Jeanette Sakel. 2004. Investigating the mechanisms of pattern replication in language convergence. Studies in Language 31(4). 829–865. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sl.31.4.05mat

Matras, Yaron & Jeanette Sakel. 2007. Introduction. In Yaron Matras & Jeanette Sakel (eds.), Grammatical borrowing in cross-linguistic perspective, 1–13. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110199192

Næss, Åshild. 2004. What markedness marks: The markedness problem with direct objects. Lingua 114. 1186–1212. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2003.07.005

Neuburger, Kathrin & Elizabeth Stark. 2014. Differential object marking in Corsican. Regularities and triggering factors. Linguistics 52(2). 365–389. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2013-0066

Piromalli, Antonio. 1996. La letteratura calabrese. Dalle origini al positivismo 1. Cosenza: Pellegrini.

Raposo, Eduardo & Juan Uriagereka. 1995. Two types of small clauses (Towards a syntax of Theme/Rheme relations). In Anna Cardinaletti & Maria Teresa Guasti (eds.), Small clauses. Syntax and semantics 28, 179–206. New York: Academic Press.

Richards, Marc. 2004. Object shift and scrambling in north and west Germanic: A case study in symmetrical syntax. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge dissertation.

Richards, Marc. 2008. Defective Agree, Case alternation and the prominence of Person. In Marc Richards & Andrej L. Malchukov (eds.), Scales, Linguistiche Arbeitsberichte 86, 137–161. Leipzig: Universität Leipzig.

Roberge, Yves. 1990. The syntactic recoverability of null arguments. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press.

Roberts, Ian. 2018. No-choice parameters, phi-features, and the structure of DP. In Ángel J. Gallego & Roger Martin (eds.), Language, syntax, and the natural sciences, 114–138. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Roeper, Tom. 1999. Universal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 2. 169–186. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728999000310

Rohlfs, Gerhard. 1969. Grammatica storica della lingua italiana e dei suoi dialetti. III. Sintassi e formazione delle parole. Turin: Einaudi.

Rohlfs, Gerhard. 1971. Autour de l’accuatif prépositionel dans les langues romanes: concordances et discordances. Revue de linguistique romane 35. 312–324.

Rohlfs, Gerhard. 1972. Nuovi scavi linguistici nella Magna Grecia. Palermo: Istituto Siciliano di Studi Bizantini e Neoellenici.

Schifano, Norma & Giuseppina Silvestri. 2017. Nuove indagini linguistiche sulla varietà greca del Salento. L’Italia Dialettale 78. 279–312.

Schwarz, David. 1977. On pragmatic presupposition. Linguistics and Philosophy 1(2). 247–257.

Schwenter, Scott. 2005. The pragmatics of negation in Brazilian Portuguese. Lingua 115. 1427–1456. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2004.06.006

Silverstein, Michael. 1976. Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In Robert Dixon (ed.), Grammatical categories in Australian languages, 112–171. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.

Sornicola, Rosanna. 1997a. Campania. In Martin Maiden & Mair Parry (eds.), The dialects of Italy, 330–337. London: Routledge.

Sornicola, Rosanna. 1997b. L’oggetto preposizionale in siciliano antico e in napoletano antico. Considerazioni su un problema di tipologia diacronica. Italienische Studien 18. 66–80.

Sornicola, Rosanna. 2011. Romance linguistics and historical linguistics: Reflections on synchrony and diachrony. In Martin Maiden, John Charles Smith & Adam Ledgeway (eds.), The Cambridge history of the Romance languages, 1–49. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CHOL9780521800723.003

Stalnaker, Robert. 1974. Pragmatic presuppositions. In Milton Munitz & Peter Unger (eds.), Semantics and Philosophy, 197–214. New York: New York University Press.

Torrego, Esther. 1998. The dependencies of objects. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Vergnaud, Jean-Roger. 2008 [1977]. Letter to Noam Chomsky and Howard Lasnik on “Filters and Control”, April 17, 1977. In Robert Freidin, Carlos P. Otero & Maria Luisa Zubizarreta (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, 3–15. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262062787.003.0002

Zamboni, Alberto. 1991. Postille alla discussione sull’accusativo preposizionale. Quaderni dell’istituto di glottologia dell’università di Chieti 3. 51–69.