1 Introduction

Differential Object Marking (DOM) is a grammatical phenomenon that marks prominent objects in many languages of the world (Bossong 1985; Aissen 2003). Object prominence is determined by several semantic and pragmatic factors related to animacy, specificity, referentiality, and focus, among others, and it is often marked overtly with specific morphology, case marking, or agreement, depending on the language. The nature of DOM has been receiving increasing attention (Bárány 2018; Börstell 2019; Ledgeway, Schifano & Silvestri 2019) because DOM exhibits synchronic and diachronic variability within and across languages. For example, among the Romance languages, Spanish, Romanian, and Sardinian have DOM, whereas Portuguese, French, and other dialects of Italian do not. In both Spanish and Romanian DOM developed diachronically. Today, some monolingual varieties of Spanish exhibit incipient expansion or extension of DOM to less prominent objects (von Heusinger & Kaiser 2005; Montrul 2013; Arechabaleta 2019; Bautista Maldonado & Montrul 2019), while in bilingual contexts, as we will see, the tendency is toward retraction, or omission, of DOM in required contexts. The variability often observed in bilingual contexts raises questions about the nature and stability of the linguistic representations of DOM in bilinguals with different levels of proficiency in the weaker language and their access to such representations during comprehension and production (Pérez-Cortés, Putnam & Sánchez 2019). In the present study we investigate knowledge of DOM in heritage speakers of Romanian in the United States.

Heritage speakers are early bilinguals whose first language is a minority language, learned before or in conjunction with the majority language of the broader society (Montrul 2016; Polinsky 2018). Because heritage speakers develop their language under conditions of reduced input during the critical period for language acquisition (Montrul, in press), the study of heritage languages offers a unique perspective on the development and use of language and the universal forces that shape it (Polinsky 2018). Heritage speakers grow up in situations where their native heritage language may not be supported by society beyond the home, and they have few opportunities for rich exposure to the language in academic and varied social contexts as they grow up. The result is that when heritage speakers reach adolescence and young adulthood they are often dominant in the majority language. As the weaker language, the heritage language retains many signatures of early native language acquisition (Montrul 2016) while also exhibiting smaller or reduced vocabularies and several grammatical divergences compared to the language of their own immigrant parents (who are full speakers of the language), or compared to the language of native speakers in the homeland (Montrul 2016; Polinsky 2018; Aalberse, Bauckus & Muysken 2019).

DOM requires integration of syntax, semantics, morphology, discourse and related interfaces (Montrul 2011; Sorace 2011; Avram & Tomescu 2020), and is susceptible to dominant language transfer in bilingualism in general. There is increasing evidence from heritage languages that this is the case: Montrul, Bhatt & Bhatia (2012), Montrul, Bhatt & Girju (2015), Bhatia & Montrul (2020), and Montrul & Bateman (2020) confirmed that obligatory DOM is often omitted by heritage speakers of Spanish, Hindi, and of Romanian in the United States. By contrast, Yager et al. (2015) and Rodríguez-Ordóñez (2017) show that DOM emerged as an innovation in heritage speakers of German in the United States, and in Basque-Spanish bilinguals in Spain. These collective findings point to the evolution and variability of DOM in situations of language contact.

In this study we further investigate the linguistic representation and use of DOM in Romanian native speakers in the homeland, first-generation immigrants, and early bilinguals (heritage speakers) of Romanian in the United States, and provide further evidence for the variability of DOM in heritage speakers of Romanian depending on type and length of the bilingual experience, as measured by production and comprehension tasks. We use multiple measures in different modalities (auditory/written) because heritage speakers typically show dissociations by language skill and modality (comprehension, production, spoken language, written language) related to their proficiency, language learning experience, and level of acquired literacy in the heritage language (Montrul 2016; Polinsky 2018; Pérez-Cortés, Putnam & Sánchez 2019). Multiple measures of linguistic ability (comprehension, production) are critical to obtain an accurate and comprehensive understanding of heritage language knowledge (Kim, O’Grady & Schwartz 2018) and access to such knowledge (Putnam & Sánchez 2013; Putnam, Pérez-Cortés & Sánchez 2019), since it is often the case that heritage speakers display different levels of accuracy depending on task modality and the degree of metalinguistic awareness required in the task (Bowles 2011; Torres 2013; Pérez-Cortés et al. 2019). Our results show that compared to studies of Spanish, DOM in the Romanian speakers tested in the present study is comparatively better preserved. When DOM and clitic omission errors occurred, these came primarily from the heritage speakers with early onset of bilingualism, consistent with findings of age effects in heritage language acquisition (Montrul 2008; 2016; Flores 2010; Ahn et al. 2017; Karayayla & Schmid 2019). Assuming a microparametric difference between Spanish and Romanian DOM (Hill & Mardale 2019; 2020), we see the few errors made by the heritage speakers as arising from slower lexical access and computation of features during production rather than from representational differences at the level of phrase and feature structure, consistent with the Differential Access Model of heritage language grammars (Pérez Cortés et al. 2019).

The next section describes DOM in Romanian and its interaction with accusative clitic doubling (CD). Section 3 covers background on the acquisition of Romanian and section 4 presents the methods and results of our study.

2 DOM in Romanian

In Romanian, animate and specific direct objects are generally doubled by an accusative clitic and marked by the preposition pe, the DOM marker. The two main parameters that regulate DOM in Romanian are animacy and referential stability (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003; Mardale 2008; 2010; Ciovârnache & Avram 2013). In Romanian, animacy is less deterministic as a trigger of DOM than it is in Spanish because specific inanimate objects can be marked, as we illustrate below. Referential stability is related to how the specificity value of a given DP can change depending on discourse properties (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003). Farkas & von Heusinger (2003) proposed a referential stability scale, as in (1).

(1) Referentiality stability scale (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003):
  Proper noouns, definite pronouns > definite DPs > partitives >indefinite DPs

Following the referentiality scale in (1), pe-marking is obligatory with personal pronouns and proper names, as in (2a,b), including those referring to personified animals (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003; Gramatica Limbii Române 2005; von Heusinger & Gáspár 2008; von Heusinger & Chiriacescu 2009). Names of cities (i.e. Chicago), are not pe-marked. Pe-marking is optional with referentially stable objects, as in modified definite animate DPs in (3) and indefinite animate DPs in (4).

    1. (2)
    1. a.
    1.   Raluca
    2.   Raluca
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-o
    2. seen-CL.3SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Beatrice/ea.
    2. Beatrice/her
    1.    ‘Raluca saw Beatrice/her.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Raluca
    2.   Raluca
    1.   a
    2. has
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. Beatrice/ea.
    2. Beatrice/her
    1.    ‘Raluca saw Beatrice/her.’
    1. (3)
    1. (L)-
    2. CL.3SG.M-
    1. am
    2. have
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. (pe)
    2. DOM
    1. băiatul
    2. boy.DEF.SG.M
    1. înalt.
    2. tall
    1. ‘I/we saw the tall boy.’
    1. (4)
    1. Roxana
    2. Roxana
    1. a
    2. has
    1. vizitat
    2. visited
    1. (pe)
    2. DOM
    1. un
    2. M.INDF
    1. prieten.
    2. friend.SG.M
    1. ‘Roxana visited a friend.’

Pe-marking is generally ungrammatical with definite specific and indefinite inanimate DPs, as in (5) and (6).

    1. (5)
    1. a.
    1.   Angelica
    2.   Angelica
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. casa.
    2. house.DEF.SG.F
    1.    ‘Angelica saw the house.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Angelica
    2.   Angelica
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. casa.
    2. house.DEF.SG.F
    1.    ‘Angelica saw the house.’
    1. (6)
    1. a.
    1.   Luminița
    2.   Luminița
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. o
    2. F.INDF
    1. casă.
    2. house.SG.F
    1.    ‘Luminița saw a house.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Luminița
    2.   Luminița
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. o
    2. F.INDF
    1. casă.
    2. house.SG.F
    1.    ‘Luminița saw a house.’

DOM (pe-marking) is also sensitive to definiteness, specificity, and dislocation (Cornilescu 2000; Mardale 2009; Țigău 2010; 2011). Strong pronominal direct objects are obligatorily marked with pe irrespective of animacy, as in (7a,b). Inanimate objects are usually not acceptable with DOM (Irimia 2018); however, specific inanimate objects can also be differentially marked with pe, as in (8). Similarly, other specific inanimate objects may trigger DOM only when dislocated as in (9a), while some speakers find that the use of DOM with inanimate objects post-verbally creates a semantic upgrading effect (9b) (Ticio & Avram 2015: 387).

    1. (7)
    1. a.
    1. L-
    2. CL.3SG.M-
    1. am
    2. have.1SG
    1. cumpărat
    2. bought
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. acesta/
    2. this.SG.M
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. celălat.
    2. other.SG.M
    1. ‘I have bought this one/the other.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Ai
    2. have.2SG
    1. luat-
    2. taken-
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. aceea.
    2. that.SG.F
    1. ‘You have taken that one.’
    1. (8)
    1. L-
    2. CL.3SG.M-
    1. ai
    2. have.2SG
    1. uitat
    2. forgot
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. A
    2. A
    1. din
    2. from
    1. text.
    2. text
    1. ‘You forgot the (letter) A in the text.’
    1. (9)
    1. a.
    1. Pe
    2. DOM
    1. trandafir
    2. rose.SG.M
    1. l-
    2. CL.3.SG.M-
    1. a
    2. have.3SG
    1. lăsat
    2. left
    1. albina
    2. bee.DEF.SG.F
    1. la
    2. at
    1. urmă.
    2. end
    1. ‘The rose, the bee left it for the end/for last.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Albina
    2. bee.DEF.SG.F
    1. l-
    2. CL.3SG.M
    1. a
    2. have.3SG
    1. lăsat
    2. left
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. trandafir
    2. rose.SG.M
    1. la
    2. at
    1. urmă.
    2. end
    1. ‘The bee left the rose for last/the end.’

Summarizing, animate specific direct objects are marked with pe in Romanian, especially if they are expressed with names or pronouns. Definite animate objects (definite DPs) are optionally pe-marked because, semantically, they are less referentially stable. In addition, there are other syntactic constraints that determine the optionality of pe-marking with definite DPs. These DPs behave differently depending on the presence or absence of the definite enclitic article, and this is the result of a syntactic constraint independent of DOM, as it applies to all prepositions in Romanian (except for cu ‘with’), such that constructions with a preposition + noun-definite article suffix are ungrammatical, as the sentence in (10) shows. Thus, if the noun is suffixed with a definite article (băiat-ul ‘boy-the’), it cannot be pe-marked unless it is further modified with an adnominal expression, as in băiatul înalt (boy-the tall) in (3) (Dobrovie-Sorin 1994; Mardale 2009; von Heusinger & Chiriacescu 2009). Some use pe and drop the definite article leaving a bare noun, as in (11a), while others drop pe and use the definite article, as in (12) (von Heusinger & Onea Gáspar 2008; von Heusinger & Chiriacescu 2009).

    1. (10)
    1. *L-
    2.   CL.3SG.M
    1. am
    2. have.1SG
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. băiatul.
    2. boy.DEF.SG.M
    1.    ‘I have seen the boy.’
    1. (11)
    1. a.
    1.   L-
    2.   CL.3SG.M
    1. am
    2. have.1SG
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. băiat.
    2. boy.SG.M
    1.    ‘I have seen the boy.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Am
    2.   have.1.SG
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. băiat.
    2. boy.SG.M
    1.    ‘I have seen the boy.’
    1. (12)
    1. Am
    2. have.1.SG
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. băiatul.
    2. boy.DEF.SG.M
    1. ‘I have seen the boy.’

When DOM is present with definite DPs, the nominal expression is referentially stable because DOM provides referential persistence in the discourse (Chiriacescu & von Heusinger 2009); when DOM is omitted, the expression has a role reading (e.g., the director, the king, the president, etc.), where the referent is not stable (Hill 2013). Some familiar functional expressions such as the mother, the teacher, the priest, the boss, etc. are exceptions to the syntactic constraint on prepositions and may appear with pe (at least in spoken Romanian) (von Heusinger & Gáspár 2008). Example (13) is acceptable under the reading that the boss is granted contextual uniqueness to its referent (role reading), though such expressions are rare.

    1. (13)
    1. L-
    2. CL.3.SG.M
    1. am
    2. have.1.SG
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. şeful.
    2. boss.DEF.SG.M
    1. ‘I have seen the boss.’

Another syntactic characteristic of Romanian is the presence of accusative clitic doubling (CD), by which the object DP is doubled by an accusative clitic. Whenever a direct object is doubled by an accusative clitic, DOM (pe-marking) is required, as in (14) and (15). Clitic doubled objects without pe-marking are typically ungrammatical (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003), but there is also some variation as we discuss below.

    1. (14)
    1. a.
    1.   Angelica
    2.   Angelica
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-
    2. seen-
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Madonna/ea.
    2. Madonna/her
    1.    ‘Angelica saw Madona/her.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Angelica
    2.   Angelica
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-
    2. seen-
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. Madonna/ea.
    2. Madonna/her
    1.    ‘Angelica saw Madona/her.’
    1. (15)
    1. a.
    1.   Elisabeta
    2.   Elisabeta
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-
    2. seen-
    1. (o)
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. o
    2. F.INDF
    1. femeie.
    2. woman.SG.F
    1.    ‘Elisabeta saw a woman.’ (specific reading)
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Elisabeta
    2.   Elisabeta
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-
    2. seen-
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. o
    2. F.INDF
    1. femeie.
    2. woman.SG.F
    1.    ‘Elisabeta saw a woman.’ (specific reading)

The distribution of CD with postverbal objects also follows the definiteness and referentiality scales: while pronouns and proper names always occur with CD, there is a strong preference for CD with pe-marked definite direct objects as in (16), and some preference for CD with pe-marked animate indefinite direct objects, as in (15) (von Heusinger & Gáspár 2008). Inanimate objects are ungrammatical with CD because CD requires DOM, and nonspecific inanimate objects cannot be pe-marked, as in (17).

    1. (16)
    1.   (L)-
    2.   CL.3.SG.M-
    1. am
    2. have.1.SG
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. bărbatul
    2. man.DEF.SG.M
    1. înalt.
    2. tall
    1.    ‘I saw the tall man.’
    1. (17)
    1. *Ioana
    2.   Ioana
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-
    2. seen-
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. o
    2. F.INDF
    1. casă.
    2. house.SG.F
    1.    ‘Ioana saw a house.’

There is some disagreement in the literature with respect to the use of CD in some contexts, particularly for definite modified objects, and this is likely due to register (higher registers prefer CD). In general, von Heusinger & Gáspár (2008), von Heusinger & Chiriacescu (2009), and Gramatica Limbii Române (2005), all indicate that CD is obligatory with proper names and personal pronouns, regardless of whether the pronouns refer to animate or inanimate objects, and thus sentences as in (18) (example from Farkas and von Heusinger 2003: 1) are ungrammatical.

    1. (18)
    1. Maria *(l)-
    2. Maria *(CL.3.SG.M)-
    1. a
    2. has
    1. desenat
    2. drawn
    1. *(pe)
    2. *(DOM)
    1. Matei/el.
    2. Matei/him
    1. ‘Maria drew Matei/him.’

With modified animate DPs, CD and DOM are optional (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003). Gramatica Limbii Române (2005) notes that CD is obligatory with modified animate DPs with strong quantifiers, while definite modified DPs are reported to be marginal for some speakers (von Heusinger & Chiriacescu 2009). For the purposes of the present study we will assume that CD and DOM are both required for proper names and personal pronouns, and optional with definite DPs, despite some variation that exists in the language. DOM and CD are ungrammatical with inanimate objects, especially if these are not specific.

Hill & Mardale (2017; 2019; 2020) propose that Romanian DOM obligatorily involves an activated bundle of discourse features in K. DOM-marked direct objects stay in situ in the VP, in a split DP structure (Bernstein et al. 2018) with two D positions (D1 and D2 and a DDOM position in between). The object marker pe spells out the discourse feature bundles of K, although it is not a functional preposition in Modern Romanian. The bundle includes the formal feature [particularized], which maps the semantic noun features animacy, reference, and specificity, and the feature [Fmark], which maps the speaker’s intention on the noun for pragmatic effects (e.g., contrastive vs. familiar effects). Diachronically, these DOM features remained in the nominal domain at all times, but changes occurred in their bundling, distribution, and spell out (Hill & Mardale 2020). So, Modern Romanian has two ways to express DOM; one with CD (the default marking option), which favors [+human] nouns, and one without CD. In Old Romanian, the DOM marker pe could check both [particularized] and [Fmark], but in Modern Romanian these features are split. The proposed structure for CD with DOM is an ApplP, as in (19). The feature [Fmark] is in the D of the DP and is checked by pe, whereas the feature [particularized] is checked by the accusative clitic in Appl.

    1. (19)

Personal pronouns and names of animate entities systematically undergo DOM through ApplP, whereas animate DPs (nouns) are only preferably derived through ApplP. Categories at the end of the referentiality scale in (1) still show traces of Old Romanian. Unmodified nouns and specific indefinites project to DP with pe merged in D, as in (20): the presence of pe forces the specific reading on the noun. Both [particularized] and [Fmark] bundled under D and mapped to pe in D.

    1. (20)

The difference between DOM pe with unmodified versus modified nouns is that, for the former, pe merges in a collapsed D, whereas in the latter pe merges in a split D field (Bernstein et al. 2018).

To summarize, Modern Romanian has two mechanisms to mark DOM: with clitic doubling and pe-marking in an ApplP, as in (19), or in a lower D position in a split DP for unmodified DPs, as in (20). In the former, the features [particularized] and [Fmark] are spelled out in separate projections by the accusative clitic and the marker pe respectively, whereas in the latter, both features are bundled and overtly expressed by the marker pe. We assume that native speakers of Romanian have and activate these two representations for DOM for comprehension and production.

3 The acquisition of DOM in monolinguals and bilinguals

Recent studies on the acquisition of DOM in different languages have found that DOM is acquired by age 3 in monolingual acquisition but it is vulnerable, with high rates of omission in required contexts, in bilingual and second language acquisition contexts (Avram 2015; Mardale & Montrul 2020). These overall trends extend to Romanian.

Following up on Rodríguez-Mondoñedo’s (2008) findings from Spanish L1 acquisition, Ticio & Avram (2015) conducted a longitudinal comparative investigation to determine whether the semantic scales of animacy and definiteness are manifested in the spontaneous production of very young children acquiring Spanish and Romanian in a monolingual context.1 The DOM systems of these two languages are very similar, both being regulated by animacy and by the referential stability scales, which separate proper names and definite pronouns from definite DPs (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003). Overall, error rates were relatively low in both languages when DOM emerged (between the ages 1;7–1;11 in Spanish and 1;9–2;2 for Romanian): 25.5% for Spanish and 13.6% for Romanian. DOM omission errors, which occurred before age 3;00, were more frequent than commission errors. In Romanian there were 13 overextensions of pe to inanimate objects, which was also noted in the child directed speech, while there was only one such extension of a to inanimate objects in Spanish (Ticio & Avram 2015: 393). The emergence and use of DOM in Spanish and Romanian followed the referentiality scale shown in (1) in the six children: the rate of DOM marking was higher for pronouns and names than for definite DPs and for indefinite DPs. Romanian CD appeared after the children produced pe and accusative clitics independently, and when CD with pe- marking appeared, it did so with names, definite pronouns, and definite DPs at the same time rather than gradually. Overall, Ticio & Avram’s (2015) findings are consistent with the relatively early mastery of DOM reported by Rodríguez-Mondoñedo (2008) for L1 Spanish.

Avram & Tomescu (2020) investigated the acquisition of DOM in Romanian-Hungarian simultaneous bilingual children living in Romania. Ticio (2015) found that DOM is very vulnerable (more than 70% omission) in Spanish-English bilingual three year-olds. Based on this finding, since Hungarian, like English, does not have an overt differential object marker (although it has different agreement on definite and indefinite objects), Hungarian-Romanian bilinguals might also show high omission rates of pe. Avram & Tomescu (2020) conducted a study with two very young bilingual children (spontaneous production) and another with older children who narrated a frog story. The findings revealed that the Hungarian-Romanian simultaneous bilinguals followed the same acquisition route with pronouns and names as Romanian monolinguals.2 However, there was more omission and instability of pe-marking with definite DPs, where DOM use involves discourse-pragmatics considerations and shows delayed acquisition.

The omission of required DOM with animate specific direct objects has been well established in the literature on Spanish as a heritage language in contact with English (Montrul & Bowles 2009; Montrul & Sánchez-Walker 2013; Montrul 2014; Arechabaleta 2019; 2020 among many others), with French (Grosjean & Py 1991), and with Dutch (Irizarri van Suchtelen 2016) and German (Pomino, Schmitz & Neuburger 2018). It has also been found in Hindi as a heritage language (Montrul, Bhatt & Bhatia 2012; Montrul, Bhatia, Bhatt & Puri 2019; Bhatia & Montrul 2020). Montrul, Bhatt & Girju (2015) examined the extent to which Romanian DOM is omitted in required contexts in adult heritage speakers of Romanian living in the United States. They found that Romanian heritage speakers accepted ungrammatical sentences with DOM omission in an acceptability judgment task, especially when these did not occur with CD. The present study seeks to verify the strength of this finding by investigating the potential of DOM omission in production and comprehension in the same adult speakers.

Many patterns of DOM omission by heritage speakers have been conceived as emerging from partial or incomplete acquisition arising from reduced or different input conditions (Montrul & Sánchez-Walker 2013), but most recent discussions on the etiology of variability in heritage language grammars seek to shift the burden of explanations away from incomplete acquisition and more toward a bilingual conception of grammars interacting at the cognitive level (Putnam & Sánchez 2013). There is growing recognition that many of the variable patterns observed in heritage speakers result from structural reanalysis and rearrangement with possible changes in representation (Polinsky 2018; Scontras, Polinsky & Fuchs 2018). Another possibility is that the syntactic structure could be intact, but the changes occur in the morphophonological exponents, as proposed by the Missing Surface Inflection Hypothesis (MSIH) (Prévost & White 2000; Lardiere 2009; Putnam, Pérez-Cortés & Sánchez 2019). Neurocognitive and psycholinguistic research on bilingualism confirms that elements of both source grammars can be simultaneously active, thus leading to the possibility that continued competition for limited processing resources may lead to a restructuring of ‘weaker’, or ‘less actively used’ grammars. In the present study, we assume and support Pérez-Cortés et al.’s (2019) position that variability in heritage language grammars arises from asymmetries in access to the lexicon, which include the functional features of syntactic projections, and to syntactic representations formed in early childhood. Infrequent use of the heritage language during childhood leads to lower proficiency in adulthood, which in turn affects the fast and efficient activation and inhibition of the features, functional projections, and morphosyntactic representations in the heritage language. Pérez-Cortés et al.’s (2019) model seeks to capture asymmetries in the comprehension and production of morphosyntactic properties of the heritage language, and to explain why morphological variability is more evident in production than in comprehension in adult heritage speakers. Variable outcomes in heritage language acquisition at high and intermediate levels of proficiency stem from reduced or inhibited access to linguistic representations during language production, which does not always arise in language comprehension because comprehension requires less activation and impulses to the neural substrate than production (Paradis 2004).

4 The Study

4.1 Research Questions and Hypotheses

Four research questions guide our study:

  1. Do heritage speakers of Romanian know the relationship between pe-DOM and CD?;

  2. Do adult heritage speakers of Romanian omit pe-marking or CD in required contexts (animate, specific direct objects that are names or pronouns) in production?;

  3. Do heritage speakers of Romanian correctly interpret pe as a direct object marker in comprehension?; and

  4. If heritage speakers of Romanian make errors, are these related to errors made by first-generation immigrants, suggesting continuity and transmission of a contact variety, or are they unrelated errors mostly arising in a few individuals with lower proficiency, suggesting that divergence arises from difficulties with representational access, and mapping of formal features to morphophonological material under cognitive load pressure?

Our study includes a cross-generational component and there are several reasons for this design. Most of the early experimental work on heritage speakers has used a group of monolingually-raised native speakers as a baseline for comparison (Montrul 2004; Montrul, Foote & Perpiñán 2008). Some have argued that heritage speakers are primarily exposed to input that may be different from the input under which monolingually raised native speakers developed their language (Sorace 2004; 2020; Rothman 2007; Pires & Rothman 2009; Pascual y Cabo 2013). Valid arguments have been made for also using a more ecologically suitable baseline group, such as, for example, the equivalent of the parental generation of the heritage speakers: namely, immigrants who grew up in the homeland and who are also now residing in the same bilingual environment as the heritage speakers (Otheguy & Zentella 2012; Kupisch & Rothman 2018; Montrul 2016; Bayram et al. 2019; Polinsky 2018). Adding a group of immigrants who speak the same contact variety as the heritage speakers allows us to address the relationship between the linguistic knowledge of the first-generation speakers and the second-generation, heritage speakers, and to trace whether structural changes in the heritage language of young adults can be related to similar patterns in the immigrant generation, under the assumption that the first-generation speakers are, generally speaking, the input providers for the heritage speakers. Furthermore, to see if the first-generation Romanian speakers speak a variety of their native language that is already influenced by contact with the majority language of the new environment, then native speakers in the homeland, who are not living in a language contact situation, must be the baseline for this group. Finally, our study also included a group of native speakers in the homeland of the same age and SES as the heritage speakers to assess convergence or potential changes in the language spoken in the homeland and in the first-generation immigrants (see also Flores, Rinke & Azevedo 2017 and Rinke, Flores & Barbosa 2018 for a similar cross-generational approach).

If the language of the adult immigrants converges with the language of the older speakers in Romania, then we can conclude that the Romanian immigrants do not manifest L1 attrition of DOM and CD. If the heritage speakers’ patterns diverge from the adult immigrants, that is, they show omission of DOM and CD and poor comprehension of DOM, unlike the first-generation immigrants and the younger and older native speakers in Romania, then such divergent uses of DOM and CD cannot be attributed to the quality of the input the heritage speakers of Romanian were exposed to in the United States. If the heritage speakers make errors, especially in production, these may be more likely due to computational difficulties and differential access to linguistic representations due to cognitive load. Computational difficulties in mapping the features [particularized] and [Fmark] to appropriate functional structure in CD+DOM (19) and DOM (20) structures may arise from early onset of bilingualism, less exposure to and use of Romanian in later childhood and adolescence, and lower proficiency in the heritage language in general.

The evidence for age effects in language attrition is substantial: younger bilingual children whose L1 is a minority language and receive less exposure and use it less than the majority language are more likely to exhibit language loss or attrition than older bilingual children and adults who had a longer period of monolingualism in their L1 (Yeni-Komshian et al. 2000; Montrul 2008; Bylund 2009; Flores 2010; Ahn et al. 2017; Karayayla & Schmid 2019). We therefore predict that the heritage speakers exposed to English and Romanian since birth (simultaneous bilinguals) and before age 5 (pre-school) will show more variability with DOM and CD than heritage speakers exposed to Romanian and then to English (sequential bilinguals) in later childhood, at around school entry, and than Romanian immigrants with age of exposure to English in adulthood.

4.2 Participants

A total of 126 Romanian speakers participated in the study. Thirty-two were Romanian immigrants and 42 were heritage speakers residing and tested in the Central Illinois and Chicago area. Fifty-two Romanian native speakers were tested in Brașov, Romania (31 were in their 20s and 21 in their 40s). The participants were divided into five groups: three from the United States—simultaneous bilingual Romanian heritage speakers (n = 23), sequential bilingual Romanian heritage speakers (n = 19), and adult Romanian immigrants (n = 32)—and two native groups from Romania: younger native speakers (n = 31) age-matched to the heritage speakers and older native speakers (n = 21) age-matched to the first-generation Romanian immigrants.

All participants completed an extensive language background questionnaire with sixty-five questions about their bilingual language history, patterns of language use across their lifespan and during schooling, self-ratings about their Romanian and English skills, attitudes toward improving Romanian, etc. We used a short Romanian proficiency test (Montrul, Bhatt & Girju 2015) as a general measure of written proficiency. Basic descriptive information about the five Romanian-speaking groups is presented in Table 1. All participants were asked to self-assess their linguistic ability in English and in Romanian, since most participants, including the native speakers tested in Romania, had been exposed to some English.

Table 1

Information about the Romanian participants in this study.

United States groups Romania groups
simultaneous bilingual HS sequential bilingual HSs adult immigrants younger native speakers older native speakers
N 20 19 30 31 21
age at testing 20.2 21.3 40.5 22.7 49.4
AoA Romanian birth birth birth birth birth
AoA English 3.33 8.7 19.2 8.4 9.3
LOR US (years) 18.1 12.1 9.2
LOR Romania 10.6 24.3 22.7 49.4
Romanian feels like an L1 41.5% 66% 100%
Romanian feels like an L2 58.5% 34%
Self-ratings in English (scale 1–5) 4.9 4.7 4.3 3.5 2.4
Self-ratings in Romanian (scale 1–5) 3.7 4.5 4.6 4.9 4.9
English listening 4.9 4.6 4.6 3.6 2.9
English speaking 4.8 4.6 4.2 3.1 2.4
English reading 4.8 4.6 4.5 3.5 2.7
English writing 4.7 4.6 4.0 2.9 2.4
Romanian listening 4.1 4.8 4.8 4.9 5
Romanian speaking 3.5 4.6 4.4 4.7 5
Romanian reading 3.7 4.5 4.3 4.8 5
Romanian writing 2.9 4.0 4.2 4.7 4.8
mean Written Prof. Test (max = 27) 22.15 24.7 26.7 26.2 26.0
SD 2.98 1.62 .59 .97 1.14
range 16–27 22–27 25–27 22–27 23–27

The immigrants immigrated after age 18, had been living in the United States between 4 and 20 years, completed their education in Romania, and had knowledge of other European languages (Hungarian, German, Russian, French, and English). The simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers were young adults between the ages of 18–23 (mean 20.2) living in the United States and born to Romanian parents. Ten of them (50%) were born in Romania and immigrated with their parents between ages 1–4 (before the onset of schooling), and the rest were born in the United States. The sequential bilingual Romanian heritage speakers were between the ages of 18 and 32, immigrated to the United States with their Romanian parents between the ages of 7 and 14, and had been living in the United States for an average of 10.6 years at the time of testing. Their mean age of acquisition of English was 8.7 years. Unlike the simultaneous bilinguals, this group had a substantial period of monolingualism in Romanian. While the 7 to 14 age of arrival range seems wide, we decided to group these speakers together because they came to the United States after they started school, whereas the four simultaneous bilinguals who moved from Romania arrived before age 5, the onset of schooling. The linguistic environment of children changes significantly with the onset of schooling, and especially for immigrant children. Carreira & Kagan (2011) report that up to age 5, heritage speakers in the United States use their heritage language 70% of the time, while after age 6, use of the heritage language drops to 18.9% and then to below 4% after 23 years of age.

The heritage speakers reported using Romanian mostly with the caregivers (mother and/or father). All the sequential bilinguals spoke Romanian with their parents. Fifteen (75%) of the simultaneous bilinguals spoke English and Romanian before age 5, the rest only Romanian. Only 8 participants (35%) lived at home with a Romanian-speaking grandparent and all of them reported speaking only Romanian with their grandparents. The parents of all the heritage speakers (simultaneous and sequential bilinguals) spoke mainly Romanian and one or more European languages (Hungarian, French, Russian, German, Italian) as second or third languages, the same languages as the immigrants. The simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers reported using mostly English with siblings and English and Romanian with the parents. The sequential bilinguals used mostly Romanian with their parents, and only 2 reported using more English. All of the simultaneous bilinguals attended English-only schools in the United States. Of the sequential bilingual group, 7 attended elementary school in Romania and the rest attended English-only schools in the United States with no Romanian as a foreign or second language at school. The simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers were exposed to more English in childhood than the sequential bilingual speakers, which may explain why these heritage speakers exhibit lower proficiency in Romanian than the sequential bilinguals and the adult immigrants. However, all the simultaneous bilinguals reported receiving between 2 and 10 hours of instruction in Romanian during their elementary school period, mostly from their parents, and no hours at all during middle school and high school.

Native speakers of Romanian were tested in Romania. Younger speakers (n = 31) were another comparison group for the heritage speakers, and the 21 older speakers were the comparison group for the immigrants. The younger Romanian speakers were between the ages of 18 and 27 and were recruited from Transylvania University of Brașov. They all had some knowledge of English and of another European language, which they had learned as a second language in Romania. The older Romanian native speakers were recruited in the same city and were between the ages of 41 and 60 (mean 49.42) at the time of testing. They were all schooled and educated in Romania and spoke some English and other European languages.

There were significant differences between the groups on their ratings of their English ability (F(4, 119) = 25.42, p < 0.0001) and their ability in Romanian (F(2, 119) = 9.89, p < 0.0001) because the two groups of heritage speakers rated their English higher than the other three groups, and their Romanian significantly lower than the other three groups. The simultaneous bilinguals and the sequential bilinguals did not differ on their self-rating of English, but they differed on their self-rating in Romanian: the simultaneous bilinguals rated their Romanian much lower than the sequential bilinguals and the difference was significant (p < 0.0001). The sequential bilinguals and the immigrants did not differ from each other on their Romanian self-ratings or their English self-ratings. Consistent with common patterns in heritage speakers, the weakest skill in the Romanian heritage speakers was writing and their strongest auditory comprehension (listening). Romanian is the weaker language of the simultaneous and sequential bilingual heritage speakers according to their self-ratings compared to their English (simultaneous bilinguals t (20) = 11.3 p < 0.0001; sequential bilinguals t(17) = 8.42, p < 0.0001), while for the immigrants their ability in the two languages was balanced and there were no statistical differences in their own self-ratings of English compared to Romanian (t(30) = 1.24, p = 0.22). For the two Romanian native speaker groups, their Romanian was stronger than their English, as expected (younger Romanians t(31) = 5.93, p < 0.0001, older Romanians t(21) = 7.17, p < 0.0001).

Because the heritage speakers had some literacy in Romanian, we deemed that using a written proficiency test was appropriate. The results of the proficiency test showed highly significant group differences (F(4, 119) = 15.18, p < 0.0001). According to Tukey post-hoc tests, the simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers scored lower than the four other groups (p < 0.0001), and the sequential bilinguals scored lower than the immigrants and the two native Romanian groups (all ps < 0.05). The immigrants and the two native Romanian-speaking groups did not differ from each other on the Romanian proficiency cloze test (all ps >0.05).

4.3 Tasks

4.3.1 Elicited Oral Production

The purpose of this task was to elicit uses of CD and pe-marking with different NPs. Participants were presented with a PowerPoint presentation with 28 pictures (one per slide) showing people performing an action with animate or inanimate objects: for example, in one image a boy is touching a plant, in another image a mother is carrying a baby. See Figure 1. The design consisted of seven verbs appearing with animate and inanimate objects (a căra ‘carry’, a cunoaşte ‘meet’, a îmbrăţişa ‘hug’, a vedea ‘see’, a vizita ‘visit’, a săruta ‘kiss’, a atinge “touch’), seven verbs that predominantly take animate objects (a primi ‘welcome’, a ataca ‘attack’, a saluta ‘greet’, a răpi, ‘kidnap’, a ajuta ‘help’, a pedepsi ‘scold’, a judeca ‘judge’), and seven verbs taking inanimate objects (a studia ‘study’, a repara ‘fix’, a bea ‘drink’, a cumpăra ‘buy’, a asculta ‘listen to’, a citi ‘read’, a semna ‘sign’). Verb and object selection were driven by picturabilty and clarity of images rather than by verb frequency. There were 28 target images (14 depicting animate objects and 14 depicting inanimate objects), and 7 fillers with psych verbs. Participants were asked to describe the pictures by forming an S-V-O sentence and conjugating the verb in the past or present tense. Describing an image that had a name as a direct object required using DOM and CD (von Heusinger & Gáspár 2008), while in describing an animate object, participants could optionally indicate DOM.

Figure 1
Figure 1

Example test items from the elicited Oral Production Task.

Target response A:

    1. (a)
    1. Profesoara
    2. teacher.DEF.F
    1. îl
    2. CL.3.SG.M
    1. ajută
    2. helps
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. copil.
    2. child.SG.M
    1. ‘The teacher helps the child.’
    1. (b)
    1. Profesoara
    2. teacher.DEF.F
    1. ajută
    2. helps
    1. copilul.
    2. child.DEF.SG.M
    1. ‘The teacher helps the child.’

Target response B:

    1.  
    1. Dan
    2. Dan
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. cară
    2. carries
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Ana.
    2. Ana
    1. ‘Dan carries Ana.’

4.3.2 Written Production Task

The written production task (WPT) consisted of 25 target sentences. Participants were given three words—a noun, a verb in the infinitive, and another noun—and were instructed to write a complete sentence with the three words given by adding all the grammatical elements they considered necessary (articles, prepositions, inflections). Five sentences targeted transitive verbs with human objects, as in (21), another five targeted sentences with inanimate objects, as in (22), and five the preposition pe as a locative (23). Another ten sentences acted as fillers and included indirect objects (24) and dative experiencers with psych verbs (25). While indirect objects are not pe-marked in Romanian, some experiencers are.4

    1. (21)
    1. animate direct object
    1.  
    1. prompt:
    1. Marisa/
    2. Marisa
    1. a cunoaşte/
    2. know
    1. sora
    2. sister.DEF.SG.F
    1. mea
    2. POSS.1.SG.F
    1.  
    1. grammatical response:
    1. Marisa
    2. Marisa
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. cunoaște
    2. knows
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. sora
    2. sister.DEF.SG.F
    1. mea.5
    2. POSS.1.SG.F
    1. ‘Marisa knows my sister.’
    1. (22)
    1. inanimate direct object
    1.  
    1. prompt:
    1. Patricio/
    2. Patricio
    1. a
    2. to
    1. vizita/
    2. visit
    1. Muzeul Prado
    2. del Prado Museum
    1.  
    1. grammatical response:
    1. Patricio
    2. Patricio
    1. a
    2. has
    1. vizitat
    2. visited
    1. muzeul
    2. museum.DEF.SG.M
    1. Prado.
    2. Prado
    1. ‘Patricio visited the del Prado Museum.’
    1. (23)
    1. pe-preposition as locative
    1.  
    1. prompt:
    1. Cartea/
    2. book
    1. a
    2. to
    1. se
    2. be
    1. afla/
    2. found/be located
    1. masă
    2. table
    1.  
    1. grammatical response:
    1. Cartea
    2. book.DEF.F
    1. se
    2. REFL.3.SG
    1. află
    2. FIND
    1. pe
    2. on
    1. masă.
    2. table
    1. ‘The book is on the table.’
    1. (24)
    1. indirect objects
    1.  
    1. prompt:
    1. Francisco/
    2. Francisco
    1. a
    2. to
    1. dezvălui/
    2. reveal
    1. mamă/
    2. mother
    1. secret
    2. secret
    1.  
    1. grammatical response:
    1. Francisco
    2. Francisco
    1. i-
    2. CL.3.SG.DAT-
    1. a
    2. has
    1. dezvăluit
    2. revealed
    1. mamei
    2. mother.DAT.SG.F
    1. un
    2. INDF.SG.M
    1. secret.
    2. secret.SG.M
    1. ‘Francisco revealed mother a secret.’
    1. (25)
    1. psych verbs (dative experiencers)
    1.  
    1. prompt:
    1. Juan/
    2. Juan
    1. a
    2. to
    1. plăcea/
    2. like
    1. Patricia
    2. Patricia
    1.  
    1. grammatical response:
    1. Juan
    2. Juan
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. place
    2. likes
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Patricia.
    2. Patricia
    1. ‘Juan likes Patricia.’

The objective of this task was to see if the heritage speakers omit pe with animate specific direct objects in written production as well, when they supposedly have more time to compose and write their responses than when speaking.

4.3.3 Auditory/Written Comprehension Task

The goal of the comprehension task was to assess whether heritage speakers perceive and assign meaning to pe-marking in DOM sentences. A picture-sentence matching task was designed to test minimal pairs differing on the presence or absence of DOM and in their argument structure. The auditory version of the task tested perception of the preposition pe (the DOM marker) after the verb, and a written version was used to determine whether heritage speakers assign meaning to the DOM marker when they see it. To minimize chance performance, the test included a third choice (a foil sentence or distractor) per minimal pair (e.g. plural: Au sunat pe Juan ‘They called Juan’) and fillers with indirect objects. (See Montrul 2014 for the Spanish version of this task).

The written and auditory versions of the picture-sentence matching task included 12 target verbs with direct objects (a invita ‘invite,’ a saluta ‘say hi,’ a suna ‘call,’ etc.), where the preposition pe is a DOM marker as shown in Figure 2, and 8 verbs with indirect objects (a cânta ‘sing,’ a scrie ‘write,’ a împușca ‘shoot,’ etc.). Each verb and each series of three pictures appeared three times, once with each sentence type (A = DOM/dative, B = V-S, C = foil). The total number of target picture-sentences was 60 (split in half, so 30 each version) and 60 fillers with indirect objects (30 each version).

Figure 2
Figure 2

Sample pictures and sentence item from the comprehension task.

    1. Prompt:
    1. A
    2. has
    1. sunat
    2. called
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Juan.
    2. Juan
    1. ‘He/she called Juan’

Participants saw three pictures (A, B, and C) on a computer screen. In picture A of Figure 2, Juan is calling somebody by phone, in picture B Juan is receiving a call, and in picture C Mary and Jane are calling Juan on the phone. At the same time, participants heard/read one sentence at a time, such as A sunat pe Juan ‘He/she called Juan’, and had to indicate which picture matched the sentence, by pressing the A, B, or C on the keyboard. In this case, B is correct. The test was completed through Survey Gizmo, a web-based survey program. After the participant made a choice, the responses disappeared: the survey was programmed so that it was not possible for participants to compare sentences and go back and change answers.

The same pictures appeared two more times in random order, with two other sentences A sunat Juan ‘Juan called.’ and Au sunat pe Juan ‘They called Juan.’ If heritage speakers do not have a representation for DOM, they may not perceive the preposition pe or assign meaning to it as a DOM marker. They will not distinguish the meaning of sentences with V-O and V-S order. They will either be at chance or they will accept more sentences without pe marking (V-S) as referring to an object: so, A sunat Juan will be interpreted as A sunat pe Juan. Thus, we expect confusion between the sentences with and without pe, or a higher error rate with the sentences with no pe and V-S order.

5 Results

5.1 Oral Production

Verbal responses were audio recorded and transcribed for analysis. Transcriptions were done by five Romanian native speakers and checked by two additional native speakers, including the second author (a speaker of Romanian). Transcripts were coded for presence and absence of pe with animate, specific direct objects, and with inanimate objects. Because referentiality is important for pe-marking in Romanian, we coded the types of DPs (definite DPs, names, pronouns, modified DPs, bare nouns), animacy, and specificity of the object. We also coded for the presence or omission of DOM and accusative clitic doubling (CD). Human definite animates such as ‘grandmother’ were coded as ‘names’ because they represent specific familial relationships in the sentences elicited and such definites have been interpreted as names in the literature (c.f. Mardale 2009; von Heusinger & Gáspár 2008; Dobrovie-Sorin & Giurgea 2013). Exactly one slide in the task elicited the noun bunica ‘grandmother’, Baiatul o vizitează pe bunica ‘The boy visits grandmother’. All other names in the task were proper names. Table A in the Appendix presents illustrative examples of how the data were coded. Because personal pronouns and names require both DOM and CD as the preferred and default option (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003; von Heusinger & Chiriacescu 2009; von Heusinger & Gáspár 2008), omission of CD with names was coded as an error.

Raw data were submitted to R (R development core team 2014) where we calculated frequencies and statistics (mixed effects logistic regressions, lmer4 package, lmertest, glmer() function in R (Baayen 2008).

The dependent variables were the presence and omission of DOM (pe) with animate and inanimate objects, and the presence or absence of CD. Errors with DOM could be of two types: omission of pe with animate, specific objects if they are pronouns and names (recall that these are referentially stable) and overextension of pe to inanimate objects that are not dislocated. Pe marking with definite DPs is optional because definite DPs are less referentially stable than pronouns and names. There were no pronouns in the data, so for animate and inanimate objects we distinguished between Names, definite DPs, indefinite DPs and definite modified DPs. Clitic doubling is correct with pronouns and proper names, but also with pe-marked bare Ns with human referents, and pe-marked modified definite DPs. CD is mostly unacceptable with inanimate objects.

We fitted several binomial logistic regression models with fixed factors as main effects or interactions. The models were built up incrementally with Group and accuracy on DOM or CD as main effects in the first model, followed with different models with interactions between different factors and group. The optimal model included the maximal random effect structure justified by the data, and this resulted in a subject intercept. We report the models that best fit the data and those that were specifically performed to address our research questions and hypotheses. Models that did not converge or which did not show any statistically significant result are not reported.

There were 3,302 observations (sentences) produced by all the participants: about half contained animate objects and the other half inanimate objects. Of these objects, 2602 (78.8%) were classified as specific, the rest non-specific (21.2%), depending on type of NP. The distribution of NP types was as follows: 21.32% names, 47.75% definite DPs, 16.11% indefinite DPs, 13.05% bare singulars (e.g., muzică ‘music’), 1.3% bare plurals. About 941 objects of 3302 (28.5%) were pe-marked. This is because the vast majority of definite DPs and 60% of bare plurals Ns were unmarked: pe-marking is optional with definite DPs. A total of 657 (19.9%) of objects had clitic doubling (CD). Figures 3 and Figures 4 show the distribution of DOM and CD presence/absence by object animacy (Figure 3) and by specificity of the object (Figure 4) by type of NP. DOM marking and CD are largely absent with inanimate objects. Not all animate objects were DOM and CD marked: overall, names were marked but definite DPs were not.

Figure 3
Figure 3

Overall Distribution of DOM and CD in the entire dataset by object animacy and NP type.

Figure 4
Figure 4

Overall Distribution of DOM and CD by object specificity and NP type in the entire dataset.

Figure 4 also shows that even though definite DPs were specific, they were unmarked because DOM is optional with definite DPs. Names, being referentially stable, were marked.

Errors were very few in general: there were 67 incorrect uses of DOM and 172 incorrect uses of clitic doubling. There was omission of DOM with specific direct objects and omission of CD with animate objects, mostly names, as shown in Figure 5.

Figure 5
Figure 5

Overall DOM accuracy by specificity and CD accuracy by animacy in the entire dataset. Group codes: ADIM: immigrants, HSE: simultaneous bilingual HS, HSL: sequential bilingual HS, NSO: older Romanian native speakers, NSY: younger Romanian native speakers.

Even though the number of errors was small, Figure 6 shows that the heritage speakers, especially the simultaneous bilingual group, produced the majority of the errors with both DOM (70% of all errors, 8.6% for the group) and CD (55% of all errors, 16.8% for the group). The sequential bilinguals made 2.9% errors with DOM and 11.5% errors with CD. Accuracy for the immigrants and the other Romanian native speaker groups ranged from 98.8–100% (ceiling).

Figure 6
Figure 6

DOM and CD accuracy by group. Group codes: ADIM: immigrants, HSE: simultaneous bilingual HS, HSL: sequential bilingual HS, NSO: older Romanian native speakers, NSY: younger Romanian native speakers.

We first ran two binomial regression analyses comparing the three native speaker groups: immigrants (ADIM), younger Romanian speakers (NSY) and older Romanian speakers on accuracy on DOM and accuracy on CD, and both analyses showed no significant differences between groups. We then compared only one of the native speaker groups—the immigrants (ADIM)—with the two groups of heritage speakers (HSE and HSL). There were significant differences between the immigrants and the heritage speakers in both DOM accuracy (Table 2) and CD accuracy (Table 3). The reference group is Immigrants.

Table 2

Fixed and Random effects of accuracy by object animacy and group.

Fixed Effects Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
(Intercept) –6.5523 0.8111 –8.079 0.0001***
OBJECTinanimate –0.7989 0.2866 –2.788 0.001**  
GroupHSE 4.0096 0.8379 4.785 0.0001***
GroupHSL 2.6087 0.8232 3.169 0.001**  
Random Effects Variance SD
1.461 1.209      
Table 3

Fixed effects CD accuracy with animate objects.

Fixed Effects Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
(Intercept) –3.7885 0.3384 –11.195 0.0001***
GroupHSE 3.0812 0.3677 8.380 0.0001***
GroupHSL 2.5513 0.3759 6.787 0.0001***
Random Effects Variance SD
0.4117 0.2029

DOM accuracy on animate objects was statistically lower than accuracy on inanimate objects. The model found significant differences between the immigrants and the heritage speakers. Pairwise comparisons showed that the immigrants were different from the two groups of heritage speakers (ADIM – HSE p < 0.0001, ADIM – HSL p < 0.0044, and the simultaneous bilinguals made significantly more errors than the sequential bilinguals (HSE- HSL, p < 0.0329).

With animate objects, the Immigrants were statistically more accurate at producing CD than the two heritage speaker groups, as confirmed by pairwise comparisons (ADIM – HSE p < 0.0001, ADIM – HSL p <.0001). As with DOM, the simultaneous bilinguals made significantly more errors with CD than the sequential bilinguals (HSE-HSL, p < 0.0200).

To summarize, we found that all speakers of Romanian are guided by the referentiality scale when marking direct objects with DOM and accusative CD in oral production. In terms of error rates, the three control groups of native speakers performed at ceiling and did not differ from each other. The heritage speakers made 8.6% omission errors of DOM with specific objects (range 75–100% accuracy) and 16.8% of omission errors of with CD (range 65.2–94.7% accuracy), mostly with names and specific definite DPs. Of the 20 simultaneous bilinguals 14 made errors with DOM and all of them made omission errors with CD). The sequential bilinguals made 2.9% errors with DOM (accuracy range 68–100%) and 11.5% errors with CD (accuracy range 73.9–100%). Of the 19 sequential bilinguals, only 4 made errors with DOM and 14 made CD omission errors). Figures 7 show the distribution of individual participants’ accuracy by group (simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers, sequential bilingual heritage speakers and adult immigrants). Sample errors are shown in (26)–(31). The dashed line at 85% marks the lowest range for the first-generation immigrants: several heritage speakers had accuracy scores that fell below the lowest accuracy score of the first-generation immigrants.

    1. (26)
    1. *Hoţul
    2.   thief.DEF.SG.M
    1. a
    2. has
    1. atacat
    2. attacked
    1. prezi… prezidente.
    2. president.SG.M
    1. Target:
    1. Hoțul
    2. thief.DEF.SG.M
    1. l-
    2. CL.3.SG.M-
    1. a
    2. has
    1. atacat
    2. attacked
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. președinte.
    2. president.SG.M
    1. ‘The thief attacked the president.’
    1. (27)
    1. *Familia
    2.   family.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. has
    1. primit
    2. received
    1. imigrant.
    2. immigrant.SG.M
    1. Target:
    1. Familia
    2. family.DEF.SG.F
    1. l-
    2. CL.3.SG.M-
    1. a
    2. has
    1. primit
    2. received
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. imigrant.
    2. immigrant.SG.M
    1. ‘The family welcomed the immigrant.’
    1. (28)
    1. *Banditul
    2.   bandit.DEF.SG.M
    1. a
    2. has
    1. răpit
    2. kidnapped
    1. femeie.
    2. woman.SG.F
    1. Target:
    1. Banditul
    2. bandit.DEF.SG.M
    1. a
    2. has
    1. răpit-
    2. kidnapped-
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. femeie.
    2. woman.SG.F
    1. ‘The bandit kidnapped the woman.’
    1. (29)
    1. *Băiatul
    2.   boy.DEF.SG.M
    1. vizită
    2. visits
    1. bunica
    2. grandmother.DEF.SG.F
    1. sa.
    2. POSS.3.SG.F
    1. Target:
    1. Băiatul
    2. boy
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. vizitează
    2. visit
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. bunica
    2. grandmother.DEF.SG.F
    1. sa.
    2. POSS.3.SG.F
    1. ‘The boy visits his grandmother.’
    1. (30)
    1. *Eduard
    2.   Eduard
    1. cunoaşte
    2. knows
    1. Alexandru.
    2. Alexandru
    1. Target:
    1. Eduard îl
    2. Eduard CL.3.SG.M
    1. cunoaște
    2. knows
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Alexandru.
    2. Alexandru
    1. ‘Eduard knows Alexandru.’
    1. (31)
    1. *David
    2.   David
    1. supără
    2. upsets/annoys
    1. Elena.
    2. Elena
    1. Target:
    1. David o
    2. David CL.3.SG.F
    1. supără
    2. upsets/annoys
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Elena.
    2. Elena
    1. ‘David upsets/annoys Elena.’

There were also several errors of use of pe with DPs with definite suffixes, such as in (32), which violated the syntactic restrictions of prepositions followed by nouns with definite articles.

    1. (32)
    1. *Profesoara
    2.   teacher.DEF.SG.F
    1. ajută
    2. helps
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. copilul.
    2. child.DEF.SG.M
    1. Target:
    1. Profesoara
    2. teacher.DEF.SG.F
    1. îl
    2. CL.3.SG.M
    1. ajută
    2. helps
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. copil.
    2. child.SG.M
    1. ‘The teacher helps the child’

Although errors were relatively few (67 incorrect uses of DOM and 172 incorrect uses of clitic doubling in the entire dataset), overall accuracy with DOM and CD was statistically significant. As predicted by our hypothesis of age effects, more simultaneous bilinguals than sequential bilinguals scored below the range of variation of the adult immigrants (below 85%). The immigrant group showed no signs of attrition (accuracy rates between 86%–100%) since they did not differ from the older and younger Romanians tested in Romania.

5.2 Written Production

The written production data was coded in the same manner as the oral production data, for presence and absence of pe with human and inanimate direct objects, indirect objects, experiencer arguments of psych verbs, and objects of preposition (locative). Correct uses of pe received 1 point and incorrect uses 0. Raw data were submitted to statistical analysis in R, a binomial logistic regression, following the same procedure and analyses as with the oral production task. Overall accuracy on the written production task was at ceiling for the three reference groups: the immigrants (99.5%) and the younger (99.6%) and older Romanian (100%) speakers. Accuracy for the sequential heritage speakers was 92.7% and 90.8% for the early bilinguals. Some heritage speakers did score below 75% accuracy with most sentences, as Table 4 shows. There were more errors in the simultaneous bilingual group than in the sequential bilingual group.

Table 4

Mean Percentage Accuracy of pe with different arguments by group in the written production task. (Ranges in parentheses).

Human object Inanimate object Indirect object Dative experiencer Locative pe
Older Romanians 99.3 (85–100) 100 100 100 100
Younger Romanians 98.9 (85–100) 100 100 100 99.2 (75–100)
Adult Immigrants 98.2 (80–100) 99.8 (94–100) 100 99.6 (97.5–100) 99.7 (90–100)
Sequential Bilingual HSs 97.8 (80–100) 99.4 (88.9–100) 100 90.6 (50–100) 92.4 (66.7–100)
Simultaneous Bilingual HSs 91.7 (50–100) 91.2 (50–100) 98.8 (75–100) 81.7 (20–100) 90.9 (66.7–100)

Different models of binomial logistic regression were run and the best model used accuracy of pe-marking as dependent variable, with group and object type as fixed factors and participants as random effect. As with the oral task, we first ran the models comparing the three control groups: immigrants, younger Romanian speakers, and older Romanian speakers, and found no significant differences between the groups. The second step was to compare the adult immigrants and the heritage speakers. Table 5 shows the fixed effects for accuracy and by type of object and group. The only significant difference was between the immigrant group and the simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers (HSE).

Table 5

Written Production Task: Effects for accuracy on type of object by group.

Fixed Effects Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
(Intercept) –4.0662 0.5823 –6.983 2.88e-12***
GroupHSE 1.7053 0.6697 2.546 0.0109*    
GroupHSL 0.2595 0.9220 0.281 0.7784      
ObjectTypeinanimate –1.5136 1.1588 –1.306 0.1915      
ObjectTypeindirect –15.4999 –792.7966 0.020 0.9844      
ObjectTypelocative –0.7701 1.1606 –0.664 0.5070      
ObjectTypepsych –0.8391 1.1604 –0.723 0.4696      
GroupHSE:ObjectTypeinanimate 1.5520 1.2351 1.257 0.2089      
GroupHSL:ObjectTypeinanimate 0.3163 1.6913 0.187 0.8517      
GroupHSE:ObjectTypeindirect 13.1422 792.7973 0.017 0.9868      
GroupHSL:ObjectTypeindirect –0.2595 1327.5310 0.000 0.9998      
GroupHSL:ObjectTypelocative 0.7728 1.2700 0.609 0.5428      
GroupHSE:ObjectTypelocative 1.9966 1.4399 1.387 0.1656      
GroupHSE:ObjectTypepsych 1.7565 1.2364 0.1554 1.421      
GroupHSL:ObjectTypepsych 2.4211 1.4128 1.714 0.0866      
Random Effects Variance SD
0.6551 0.8094

Some examples of errors produced by the heritage speakers appear in (33), (34), and (35).

    1. (33)
    1. *Sora
    2.   sister.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. to
    1. vedea
    2. see
    1. Carmen
    2. Carmen
    1. ieri.
    2. yesterday
    1. Target:
    1. Sora
    2. sister.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-o
    2. seen-CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Carmen
    2. Carmen
    1. ieri.
    2. yesterday
    1. ‘The sister saw Carmen yesterday.’
    1. (34)
    1. *Studenta
    2.   student.DEF.SG.F
    1. vizita
    2. visits
    1. Sabina.
    2. Sabina
    1. Target:
    1. Studenta
    2. student.DEF.SG.F
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. vizitează
    2. visits
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Sabina.
    2. Sabina
    1. ‘The student visits Sabina.’
    1. (35)
    1. *Mama
    2.   mother.DEF.SG.F
    1. fascina
    2. fascinate
    1. Luciano
    2. Luciano
    1. Pavarotti.
    2. Pavarotti
    1.    ‘Mother fascinates Luciano Pavarotti.’
    1. Target:
    1. Pe
    2. DOM
    1. mama
    2. mother.SG.F
    1. o
    2. CL.3.SG.F
    1. fascinează
    2. fascinates
    1. Luciano
    2. Luciano
    1. Pavarotti.
    2. Pavarotti
    1. ‘Luciano Pavarotti fascinates Mother.’

There were very few overextensions of pe to inanimates, as in (36) and (37).

    1. (36)
    1. *Patricio
    2.   Patricio
    1. visiteasa
    2. visits
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Muzeul
    2. Museum.DEF.SG.M
    1. Prado.
    2. Prado
    1. Target:
    1. Patricio
    2. Patricio
    1. vizitează
    2. visits
    1. Muzeul
    2. Museum.DEF.SG.M
    1. Prado.
    2. Prado
    1. ‘Patricio visits Del Prado Museum.’
    1. (37)
    1. *Bărbatul
    2.   man.DEF.SG.M
    1. a
    2. has
    1. ascultat
    2. listened
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. partida
    2. match.DEF.SG.F
    1. de
    2. of
    1. fotbal
    2. soccer
    1. de la
    2. from
    1. Brazilia.
    2. Brazil
    1. Target:
    1. Bărbatul
    2. man.DEF.SG.M
    1. a
    2. has
    1. ascultat
    2. listened
    1. partida
    2. match.DEF.SG.F
    1. de
    2. of
    1. fotbal
    2. soccer
    1. cu
    2. with
    1. Brazilia.
    2. Brazil
    1. ‘The man listened to the soccer match with Brazil.’

There were no errors of omitting pe with locatives, as most of the errors with locatives involved use of the wrong preposition (e.g. in instead of pe) (*Pisica a dormit în podea ‘The cat slept in the floor’, or *Tata a sta în un scaun ‘Father sat on/in a chair’), which might be a transfer from English as the preposition în is closest in form to the English prepositions in and on.

In this written task as well, we coded for incorrect uses of CD, especially omission with human objects of transitive verbs and experiencers of psych verbs. As in the oral production task, the simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers produced 17.7% of clitic omission errors (82.3% accuracy) and the sequential bilingual heritage speakers produced 6.6% errors (93.4% accuracy). Compared to the reference group of immigrants who were at ceiling (99.2% accuracy), these error rates were statistically significant. The results of the binomial logistic regression for CD accuracy are in Table 6. Pairwise comparisons were significant at p < 0.0001 among the three groups.

Table 6

Written production task: Fixed effects of CD accuracy by group.

Fixed Effects Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
(Intercept) –4.9040 0.4187 –11.714 0.0001***
GroupHSE 3.3130 0.4398 7.534 0.0001***
GroupHSL 2.1850 0.4642 4.707 0.0001***
Random Effects Variance SD
0.1348 0.3671

Examples of errors with clitic omission in the written task are (38) and (39).

    1. (38)
    1. *Soara
    2.   sister.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut
    2. seen
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Carmen
    2. Carmen
    1. ieri.
    2. yesterday
    1. Target:
    1. Sora
    2. sister.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. has
    1. văzut-o
    2. seen-CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Carmen
    2. Carmen
    1. ieri.
    2. yesterday
    1.    ‘The sister saw Carmen yesterday.’
    1. (39)
    1. *Studenta
    2.   student.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. has
    1. vizitat
    2. visited
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Sabina.
    2. Sabina
    1. Target:
    1. Studenta
    2. student.DEF.SG.F
    1. a
    2. has
    1. vizitat-o
    2. visited-CL.3.SG.F
    1. pe
    2. DOM
    1. Sabina.
    2. Sabina
    1. ‘The student visited Sabina.’

Thus, as predicted, when errors were made, these came from the Romanian heritage speakers with exposure to English from a younger age (simultaneous bilinguals).

5.3 Comprehension

Correct answers on the written and auditory comprehension tasks received a 1 and incorrect 0. The accuracy data were submitted to a binomial logistic regression, following the same procedure as in the two other tasks. The best model included accuracy by group, and modality and sentence type as fixed factors with participants as random factor. Because there were no differences between the three native speaker groups, only the immigrant group was used as reference group for this analysis. All participant groups were very accurate on this task, and modality (written vs. auditory versions) did not affect the results. As Table 7 shows, all participants had higher accuracy with the indirect objects (filler) and plural sentences than with V-DOM and V-S sentences, which involved postverbal subjects. Of all the groups, the simultaneous bilinguals were more inaccurate than the sequential bilinguals and the immigrant group with V-S sentences.

Table 7

Percentage Accuracy on sentence types by group in the Comprehension Task.

V-DOM V-S plural Indirect Object
Older Romanians 88.7 86.2 92.7 94.6
Younger Romanians 88.5 84.7 94.6 94.4
Adult Immigrants 89.2 87.4 92.1 92.5
Sequential Bilingual HSs 88.1 85.2 91.2 92.7
Simultaneous Bilingual HSs 84.6 78.1 91.4 92.1

The logistic regression model shown in Table 8 found no effect for group or modality. All groups made errors with V-DOM and V-S sentences compared to plural sentences, and the simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers were more inaccurate with these sentences (21.9% error rate) than the other groups. This means that they were choosing pictures depicting objects instead of subjects in V-S sentences without pe more than pictures depicting subjects in the sentences with pe-marked objects. Their range of accuracy with both V-DOM and V-S sentences was 46 –100%, which means that some participants were at chance. Although errors in this task were very few, most errors were made by the heritage speakers with less exposure to Romanian since childhood.

Table 8

Effects for accuracy on the comprehension task by task modality, sentence type and group.

Fixed Effects Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
(Intercept) 2.829913 0.243791 11.608 0.0001***
GroupSEQ –0.048328 0.369341 –0.131 0.895895      
GroupSIM –0.066856 0.374215 –0.179 0.858207      
SentenceTypeVDOM –0.359745 0.179352 –2.006 0.044877*    
SentenceTypeVS –0.636874 0.173042 –3.680 0.000233***
ModalityW 0.007444 0.085252 0.087 0.930423      
GroupSEQ:SentenceTypeVDOM –0.019146 0.275853 –0.069 0.944666      
GroupSIM:SentenceTypeVDOM –0.429868 0.272872 –1.575 0.115176      
GroupSEQ:SentenceTypeVS –0.032488 0.266488 –0.122 0.902969      
GroupSIM:SentenceTypeVS –0.620854 0.262569 –2.365 0.018053*    
Random Effects Variance SD
1.074 1.037

6 Discussion

Previous studies of DOM in heritage languages, and in situations where the majority language does not instantiate DOM, have found divergence and variability in the uses of DOM in the form of omission of DOM with animate and specific direct objects. This study sought to ascertain the vulnerability of DOM in Romanian heritage speakers in the United States. The main research questions guiding our study were 1) whether heritage speakers know the relationship between pe-DOM and CD 2) whether adult heritage speakers of Romanian omit pe-marking in required contexts (animate, specific direct objects that are names or pronouns) in oral and written production; 3) whether heritage speakers correctly interpret pe as a direct object marker in comprehension; 4) whether errors with DOM and CD made by the heritage speakers of Romanian were related to similar errors made by immigrants, suggesting continuity and transmission of a contact variety, or due to difficulties with lexical and representational access arising in a few individuals with lower proficiency.

We investigated these questions in a cross-generational design that included heritage speakers with different ages of exposure to the majority language, English (early in simultaneous bilinguals and later in sequential bilinguals), and first-generation immigrants to the United States. Because the linguistic knowledge of heritage speakers is manifested differently by language skill (comprehension, production) and modality of tasks, we used elicited oral and written production and off-line comprehension tasks.

Our results found overall high performance in all tasks: the immigrants and the heritage speaker groups showed much less variability of DOM compared to adult immigrants and heritage speakers of Spanish (Montrul 2014). First, we found no signs of attrition in the adult immigrant group, as this group’s responses converged in all the production and comprehension measures with those of the native speakers tested in Romania, both younger and older, who performed largely at ceiling in all three tasks, above 90% accuracy. All the adult immigrants, except one who performed at 86% accuracy with DOM and CD in oral production, performed above 90% accuracy. Assuming the structures for CD+DOM in (19) and DOM in (20), these results suggest that the Romanian immigrants do not exhibit attrition or inefficient and slow lexical access to pe and clitics for constructing a syntactic representation for DOM and CD in Romanian (Putnam et al. 2019).

The heritage speakers were more accurate than what has been reported for speakers of Spanish or Hindi as a heritage language, suggesting that they may also have similar syntactic representations to the monolinguals and the Romanian immigrants. At the same time there was more internal variability in the heritage groups than in the other groups as more heritage speakers exhibited divergent responses in all three tasks than in the other groups. That is, 17 of 20 heritage speakers exposed to English earlier in childhood (simultaneous bilinguals) made between 35%–12% errors (i.e., scored below 90% the lower bound for all the other native speaker groups) in the oral production task. These were significantly more errors than for the heritage speakers exposed to English later in childhood (sequential bilinguals) (7 of 19 made between 35–12% errors). Since monolingual Romanian children make less than 13.6% of errors with omission of DOM in required contexts before age 3;00 (Ticio & Avram 2015), an error rate above 15% for adult heritage speakers when native speakers and immigrants make almost none, cannot be dismissed as noise. (See also individual results in Figure 7). In the three tasks, the immigrants were statistically significantly different from the simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers, and in the two production tasks they were also significantly different from the sequential bilingual heritage speakers. These findings suggest that the few omissions of DOM and CD in required contexts produced by the heritage speakers do not arise from similar omission patterns present in the parental input (Sorace 2004; 2020; Pascual y Cabo 2013; Kupisch & Rothman 2018). Instead these errors may arise form slow access to the lexical elements and inefficient building of syntactic representations, related to lower proficiency than the other groups, in turn related to earlier age of onset of bilingualism, and less input in Romanian during older childhood (Flores 2010; Montrul 2018).

Figure 7
Figure 7

Individual participants’ mean accuracy production of pe-marking (DOM) and clitic doubling (CD) with specific direct objects.

As predicted by Pérez-Cortés et al. (2019)’s Differential Access Model, we found dissociations in accuracy in comprehension and production for some of the heritage speakers, who were more accurate with DOM in comprehension than in production. There are two possible reasons for the production-comprehension dissociation found in our study: one is the tasks, the other the grammar. In the production tasks, participants were given the words and they had to verbally construct sentences adding the necessary morphology. In the comprehension task, they were given three pictures and they had to match a very short simple sentence with one of the pictures. It takes more processing resources to access the lexicon, activate the relevant features, bundle them and map them to morphophonological forms to produce sentences with the required morphology under time pressure in an experimental task (especially if it is oral) than to comprehend sentences when all the words are given. Language production is bottom-up processing, while language comprehension is top-down, relies on heuristics, and requires less cognitive resources (Paradis 2004). The fact that heritage speakers in this study had more errors in production than in the comprehension/written tasks supports the observation that heritage speakers exhibit online processing limitations when using their weaker language (Polinsky 2018; Pérez-Cortés et al. 2019; Putnam et al. 2019), despite their relatively advanced proficiency.

It can also be argued that the comprehension task was very easy and the results, therefore, not representative of the heritage speakers’ knowledge. The comprehension task used in the present study is a version of the same task used with Spanish heritage speakers and immigrants in the Montrul (2014) study. The error rate reported by Montrul (2014) was higher than the error rate in the present study, which suggests that the task is not necessarily that easy. In general, we can say that the heritage speakers do have grammatical knowledge of DOM and CD in Romanian, but a few individuals in the heritage groups may have had difficulty accessing lexical and grammatical knowledge (including formal features and the projections where they are checked in the DP or ApplP), or integrating operations related to feature bundling, feature expression, and feature checking during oral and written production. These components of the grammar are activated and accessed differently for comprehension, since comprehension is a top-down driven process. There were also very few errors in the comprehension task compared to the production tasks, and these came from the same simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers who also performed the least accurately in the production tasks. Therefore, for a handful of individuals who scored consistently below the range of native speakers and the adult immigrants on the different tasks, a grammatical explanation is warranted.

Overall, the distribution of DOM and CD in the data is consistent with the description of DOM and CD in the Romanian syntactic and semantic literature (Farkas & von Heusinger 2003; Ciovârnache & Avram 2013; Mardale 2008; 2010; Hill & Mardale 2019; 2020), but the heritage speakers who omitted pe-in required contexts in the production tasks also omitted CD. Many other heritage speakers omitted CD but not pe, which is still possible (although dispreferred) in Romanian, as discussed in section 2. We found that DOM was optional with animate DPs, which are more referentially unstable than names according to Farkas & von Heusinger’s (2003) referentiality scale in (1), and the majority of errors were of omission of DOM and CD with names. (There were no pronouns in the data).6

According to Paradis’ (2004) Activation Threshold Hypothesis, production requires more neural impulses than comprehension, which explains why for many bilinguals who do not use the language frequently, production is more difficult than comprehension. For the cases of CD omission in CD+DOM contexts, one possibility is that heritage speakers who made these errors access the split feature ApplP analysis in (19) for names and pronouns efficiently or consistently, but due to cognitive pressure and slow lexical retrieval, they fail to spell out [particularized] overtly through the clitic in ApplP, a surface morphological problem (Putnam et al. 2019). Pe is retained and spelled out in these cases because it has semantic features, whereas the clitic has discourse features.

    1. (19)

Recall that unmodified nouns and specific indefinites project to DP with pe merged in D, as in (20), where both [particularized] and [Fmark] bundle under D and are mapped to pe in D. The other possibility is that because both the ApplP in (19) and the DP in (20) are available for DOM, some heritage speakers, under communicative and cognitive pressure, activate the DP (available for unmodified nouns) instead of the ApplP to check DOM for both names and modified DPs, bundling the features [particularized] and [Fmark] under pe (as was done in Old Romanian).

    1. (20)

So, even though Modern Romanian offers two possible structures for DOM (one with feature splitting and one with feature bundling), heritage speakers, sometimes, under cognitive pressure or inefficient lexical and structural access, use only one of the available structures for names and DPs. As proposed by Scontras et al. (2018) for representational changes in gender agreement in Spanish heritage speakers, the pressures to establish minimal domains explains the bundling of the features [personalized] and [Fmark] under one head (D) by the lower-proficiency Romanian heritage speakers in the present study. When both pe and CD are omitted, the heritage speakers may still access the ApplP in (19) or the DP in (20), but fail to realize the marking overtly, again following Putnam et al. (2019). The fact that the errors attested are more likely to arise from lexical and representational access, rather than by a simpler structural or featural representation, is supported by the fact that the comprehension of CD and DOM-marked names and DPs was almost at ceiling for most speakers, with a very few exceptions.

Still, compared to what has been found in several studies of Spanish heritage speakers, including young bilingual children (Ticio 2015), older children and adults (Montrul & Sánchez-Walker 2013), the findings we have so far from Romanian L1 acquisition (Ticio & Avram 2015), Hungarian-Romanian bilinguals (Avram & Tomescu 2020), and now adult Romanian heritage speakers in the United States, suggest that the vulnerability of DOM in Romanian in contact with English is much less extensive than the divergence and potential language change observed with DOM in Spanish in contact with English (Montrul 2014; Montrul, Bhatt & Girju 2015). We also did not find in this study differences between the performance of the heritage speakers in spoken vs. written modality of the tasks, as has been found in other heritage speaker groups (Bowles 2011; Torres 2013).

There are two potential linguistic explanations for why DOM in Romanian heritage speakers and immigrants is apparently less vulnerable, overall, than DOM in Spanish heritage speakers and immigrants. One possibility is the phonological shape of the marker, which is the “a” in Spanish but “pe” in Romanian. The issue of perceptual salience in the acquisition and retention of morphology in heritage grammars has been discussed at length by Kim, O’Grady & Schwartz (2018), Montrul, Bhatt & Girju (2015), and Polinsky (2018). The idea is that morphemes that are more perceptually salient are acquired and retained better than morphemes that are acoustically less salient. In Spanish, many verbs in the 3rd person singular present tense of the -ar conjugation (ayudar ‘help’) end in the vowel /a/, and the DOM marker is also /a/ (Ayuda a Juan ‘He/she helps Juan’). Being a canonical CV syllable, Romanian “pe” seems to be more acoustically perceptible than “a” and therefore more noticeable. If acoustic salience is an issue (Polinsky 2018: 165–169), it is not surprising to see that Romanian DOM is preserved more than Spanish DOM in heritage speakers. Therefore, it is likely that the structural properties of Romanian (perceptual salience of the marker and co-occurrence of DOM with accusative CD) may contribute to the higher preservation of DOM in Romanian heritage speakers compared to Spanish heritage speakers.

Other reasons for why the Romanian heritage speakers have relatively good command of Romanian could be extralinguistic, related to the fact that Romanian families appear to provide literacy opportunities to their children at home, especially because their children will not have access to the heritage language at school. We gathered this information from the language background questionnaire administered to the heritage speakers, where there were several questions about use of Romanian in the home and the number of interlocutors. The heritage speakers reported speaking Romanian with their parents and friends. They reported using Romanian often or seldom, and it was mostly when speaking with their parents and siblings, although they frequently used it with English. When asked whether parents read to them in Romanian growing up, 80% of the sequential bilinguals and 65% of the simultaneous bilinguals were read to in Romanian. We also asked whether parents corrected them when they made mistakes in Romanian and for 90% of the simultaneous bilinguals and 75% of the sequential bilinguals the answer was positive. These reports indicate that there was some exposure to early literacy in the home and that the parents promoted target-like use of Romanian. The fact that the immigrant group, who were the proxy of the heritage speakers’ parents, does not seem to show signs of attrition suggests that the input heritage speakers are exposed to is target-like. Therefore, in addition to the structure of the language, there are several potential other external factors related to attitudes, efforts for language maintenance, and exposure to the language in the families that may explain the overall high command of Romanian by the heritage speakers and the immigrant groups.

7 Conclusion

This study joins many others in showing that DOM is a variable and vulnerable linguistic phenomenon diachronically and in situations of language contact. DOM is vulnerable to omission in heritage language speakers who are dominant in English, a language that does not mark DOM. CD is related to DOM in Romanian and is also vulnerable to omission with human specific objects. Consistent with other studies of bilingual heritage speakers, the results of this study add further evidence for the role of age of acquisition in the morphological variability exhibited by heritage speakers: most errors were made by simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers compared to sequential bilinguals. Finally, there was no evidence of language attrition in the adult, immigrant group, suggesting that divergences found in the grammars of heritage speakers with respect to DOM cannot be traced back from how the language is used in the diaspora that is also affected by language contact, at least in the specific population tested in the present study.

Additional File

The additional file for this article can be found as follows:

Table A

Examples of coded data. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.1135.s1

Abbreviations

1 = first person

2 = second person

3 = third person

CL = clitic

DEF = definite

DOM = Differential Object Marking/marker

F = feminine

M = masculine

POSS = possessive

REFL = reflexive

SG = singular

Ethics and Consent

The study was approved by the University of Illinois Institutional Review Board before any data were collected and all participants read and signed written consent forms before participation.

Notes

  1. The Animacy Hierarchy (Silverstein 1976)
    human > animate > inanimate
    Definiteness Scale (Croft 1988; Aissen 2003: 437)
    pronoun> Proper name > Definite NP > indefinite specific NP > Non-specific NP.
    [^]
  2. If differential agreement for animate and inanimate objects is a kind of DOM, then the children could have transferred DOM from Hungarian. [^]
  3. There were 4 participants who lived in Romania until age 5, the rest all were born in the United States. For Meisel (2013), age 4 is already a sequential bilingual. Our reasoning for including these individuals in the simultaneous bilingual group was that 5 years old is still pre-school; the sequential bilinguals, who were older than 7 and 8 when they arrived in the United States, had experience with school in Romanian. Because age 5 is critical for heritage language children due to the drastic change in input upon school entry, we therefore included these early bilinguals in the simultaneous bilingual group. [^]
  4. Because these items were fillers we do not expand on the syntax of indirect objects and psych verbs in the present study. [^]
  5. Only two of the five stimuli in this category included possessive NPs. The other three were names. [^]
  6. We also found that some heritage speakers made errors with the syntactic restriction on preposition + noun-definite article and there were very few extensions of pe to inanimate objects. These errors came mostly from the simultaneous bilingual heritage speakers (*Bratra… bra… batranul a tinut [pe un umbrela] ‘The old man held [DOM an umbrella]’). [^]

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the Romanian participants in the study, to Roxana Girju for her contributions to the development of the test instruments, and to the Romanian research assistants Raluca Kim, Luminita Marcus, Andreea Faur, Bogdan Buricea, Codruta Girlea and Daniela Raducanu, who helped with data collection and transcriptions of the Romanian data. We also thank Marissa Barlaz for guidance for the statistical analyses. The results and implications of this study were discussed at length with Klaus von Heusinger and with Alexandru Mardale and we are also grateful for their feedback at earlier stages of this process. Finally, we thank the three anonymous reviewers of Glossa for their insightful and helpful suggestions to improve our study. All remaining errors are our responsibility.

Funding Information

This material is based in part upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant Number BCS-0917593, ARRA to Silvina Montrul, Rakesh Bhatt and Roxana Girju (2009–2013). Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Author Contributions

Silvina Montrul designed and conducted the study and supervised the research assistants who collected and analyzed the data. Nicoleta Bateman checked all audio files and corrected transcriptions done by six research assistants, defined and completed the data coding, and managed data preparation for statistical analysis. Silvina Montrul (with help of data analyst Marissa Barlaz) conducted the statistical analysis. Both Silvina Montrul and Nicoleta Bateman discussed the data and co-wrote the paper.

References

Aalberse, Suzanne, Ad Bauckus & Pieter Muysken. 2019. Heritage languages: A language contact approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.58

Ahn, Sunyoung, Charles Chang, Robert DeKeyser & Sungyoung Lee-Ellis. 2017. Age effects in first language attrition: Speech perception by Korean-English bilinguals. Language Learning 67. 694–733. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12252

Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21. 435–483. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024109008573

Arechabaleta, Begoña. 2019. The processing of differential object marking in Spanish by monolinguals and bilinguals. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois.

Arechabaleta, Begoña. 2020. The processing of differential object marking by heritage speakers of Spanish. In Alexandru Mardale & Silvina Montrul (eds.), The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking, 237–260. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.26.are09

Avram, Larissa. 2015. Editorial: The L1 Acquisition of differential object marking. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique [Romanian Journal of Linguistics] LX 4. 331–338. Bucureşti.

Avram, Larissa & Veronica Tomescu. 2020. Differential object marking in simultaneous Hungarian-Romanian bilinguals. In Alexandru Mardale & Silvina Montrul (eds.), The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking, 77–104. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.26.avr03

Baayen, R. Harald. 2008. Analysing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801686

Bárány, András. 2018. DOM and dative case. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3(1): 97. 1–40. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.639

Bautista Maldonado, Salvador & Silvina Montrul. 2019. An experimental investigation of differential object marking in Mexican Spanish. Spanish in Context 16. 22–50. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sic.00025.bau

Bayram, Fatih, Jason Rothman, Michael Iverson, Tanja Kupisch, David Miller, Eloi Puig-Mayenco & Marit Westergaard. 2019. Differences in use without deficiencies in competence: passives in the Turkish and German of Turkish heritage speakers in Germany. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 22. 919–939. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2017.1324403

Bernstein, Judith, Francisco Ordóñez & Francesc Roca. 2018. DOM and DP layers in Romance. Paper presented at the International Workshop DOM in Romance—Toward Microvariation. Paris, INALCO, November 9–10.

Bhatia, Archna & Silvina Montrul. 2020. Comprehension of differential object marking by Hindi heritage speakers. In Alexandru Mardale and Silvina Montrul (eds.) The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking, 161–182. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.26.bha10

Börstell, Carl. 2019. Differential object marking in sign languages. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 4(1): 3. 1–18. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.780

Bossong, Georg. 1985. Empirische Universalienforschung [Empirical research on universals]. Differentielle Objecktmarkierung in den neuiranischen Sprachen [Differential object marking in the modern Iranian languages]. Tübingen: Narr.

Bowles, Melissa. 2011. Measuring implicit and explicit linguistic knowledge: what can heritage language learners contribute? Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33. 247–272. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263110000756

Bylund, Emanuel. 2009. Maturational constraints and first language attrition. Language Learning 59. 687–715. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00521.x

Carreira, Maria & Olga Kagan. 2011. The results of the national heritage language survey: implications for teaching, curriculum design, and professional development. Foreign Language Annals 44. 40–64. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.2010.01118.x

Chiriacescu, Sofiana & Klaus von Heusinger. 2009. Discourse prominence and pe-marking in Romanian. International Review of Pragmatics 2. 298–332. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1163/187731010X528377

Ciovârnache, Cristina & Larisa Avram. 2013. Specificity and animacy in the acquisition of differential object marking in L2 Persian. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique LVIII 4. 417–436. Bucureşti.

Cornilescu, Alexandra. 2000. On the interpretation of the prepositional accusative in Romanian. Bucharest Working Papers in Linguistics 2. 91–106.

Croft, William. 1988. Agreement vs. case marking and direct objects. In Michael Barlow & Charles Ferguson (eds.), Agreement in Natural Languages: Approaches, Theories, Descriptions, 159–179. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information.

Dobrovie-Sorin, Carmen. 1994. The syntax of Romanian. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110886597

Dobrovie-Sorin, Carmen & Ion Giurgea. 2013. A reference grammar of Romanian. Volume 1: The noun phrase. John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/la.207

Farkas, Donka & Klaus von Heusinger. 2003. Stability of reference and object marking in Romanian. Paper presented at Workshop on Direct Reference and Specificity, ESSLLI, Vienna, August 2003.

Flores, Cristina. 2010. The effect of age on language attrition: Evidence from bilingual returnees. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13. 533–546. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S136672890999054X

Flores, Cristina, Esther Rinke & Cecília Azevedo. 2017. Object realization across generations: A closer look on the spontaneous speech of Portuguese first and second generation migrants. In Elisa Di Domenico (ed.), Syntactic Complexity from a Language Acquisition Perspective, 178–205. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Gramatica Limbii Române. 2005. [The Grammar of Romanian]. Vol I. Cuvântul. Bucureşti: Editura Academiei.

Grosjean, François & Bernard Py. 1991. La restructuration d’une première langue: L’intégration de variantes de contact dans la compétence de migrants bilingües [Reconstructing a first language: Integrating contact variants into the competence of bilingual migrants]. La Linguistique 27. 35–60.

Hill, Virginia. 2013. The direct object marker in Romanian: a historical perspective. Australian Journal of Linguistics 33.140–151. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/07268602.2013.814527

Hill, Virginia & Alexandru Mardale. 2017. On the interaction of differential object marking and clitic doubling in Romanian. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique LXII 4. 393–409. Bucureşti.

Hill, Virginia & Alexandru Mardale. 2019. The internal structure of a differentially marked DP in Romanian. Bucharest Working Papers in Linguistics 21. 87–107. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.31178/BWPL.21.1.3

Hill, Virginia & Alexandru Mardale. 2020. The diachrony of differential object marking in Romanian. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Irimia, Monica. 2018. When differential object marking is obligatory: Some remarks on the role of case in ellipsis and comparatives. U. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 24(1). 105–114.

Irizarri van Suchtelen, Pablo. 2016. Spanish as a heritage language in the Netherlands. A cognitive linguistic exploration. Utrecht: LOT Dissertation.

Karayayla, Tugba & Monika Schmid. 2019. First language attrition as a function of age of onset of bilingualism: First language attainment of Turkish-English bilinguals in the United Kingdom. Language Learning 69. 106–142. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12316

Kim, Kitaek & William O’Grady & Bonnie Schwartz. 2018. Case in heritage Korean. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8. 252–282. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16001.kim

Kupisch, Tanja & Jason Rothman. 2018. Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism 22. 564–582. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916654355

Lardiere, Donna. 2009. Some thoughts on the contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research 25. 173–227. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0267658308100283

Ledgeway, Adam, Norma Schifano & Giuseppina Silvestri. 2019. Differential object marking and the properties of D in the dialects of the extreme south of Italy. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 4(1): 51. 1–25. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.569

Mardale, Alexandru. 2008. Microvariation within differential object marking: Data from Romance. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique 53. 448–467. Bucureşti.

Mardale, Alexandru. 2009. Les prépositions fonctionnelles du roumain: études comparatives sur le marquage casuel [Functional prepositions in Romanian: comparative studies on casual marking]. Sémantiques. Paris: L’Harmattan.

Mardale, Alexandru. 2010. Éléments d’analyse du marquage différentiel de l’objet dans les langues romanes [Elements of analysis of the differential object marking in Romance languages]. Faits de Langues – Les Cahiers 2. 161–197. Paris: Ophrys. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1163/19589514-035-036-02-900000008

Mardale, Alexandru & Silvina Montrul (eds.). 2020. The acquisition of differential object marking. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.26

Meisel, Jürgen. 2013. Sensitive phases in successive bilingual acquisition: The critical period hypothesis revisited. In Cédric Boeckx & Kleanthes Gromann, (eds.) The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics, 69–85. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980435.007

Montrul, Silvina. 2004. Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morpho-syntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7. 125–142. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728904001464

Montrul, Silvina. 2008. Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.39

Montrul, Silvina. 2011. Interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua 212. 591–604. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.05.006

Montrul, Silvina. 2013. Differential object marking in Argentine Spanish: An experimental study. In Laura Colantoni & Celeste Rodríguez Louro (eds.). The Handbook of Argentine Spanish. Frankfurt: Vervuert Iberoamericana, 207–228.

Montrul, Silvina. 2014. Searching for the roots of structural changes in the Spanish of the United States. Lingua 151. 177–196. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2014.05.007

Montrul, Silvina. 2016. The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139030502

Montrul, Silvina (in press). Heritage language speakers inform the Critical Period Hypothesis for first and second language acquisition. In Casilde Isabelli & Tania Leal Méndez (eds.). Title TBD. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Montrul, Silvina & Nicoleta Bateman. 2020. Differential object marking in Romanian as a heritage language. In Alexandru Mardale & Silvina Montrul (eds.), The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking, 283–312. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.26.11mon

Montrul, Silvina & Melissa Bowles. 2009. Back to basics: Differential object marking under incomplete acquisition in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12. 363–383. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990071

Montrul, Silvina & Noelia Sánchez-Walker. 2013. Differential object marking in child and adult Spanish heritage speakers. Language Acquisition 20. 109–132. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.766741

Montrul, Silvina, Rakesh Bhatt & Archna Bhatia. 2012. Erosion of case and agreement in Hindi heritage Speakers. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2. 141–176. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.2.2.02mon

Montrul, Silvina, Rakesh Bhatt & Roxana Girju. 2015. Differential object marking in Spanish, Hindi and Romanian as heritage languages. Language 91. 564–610. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2015.0035

Montrul, Silvina, Rebecca Foote & Silvia Perpiñán. 2008. Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning 58. 503–553. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2008.00449.x

Montrul, Silvina, Archna Bhatia, Rakesh Bhatt & Vandana Puri. 2019. Case in Hindi as a weaker language. Frontiers in Psychology 10. 461. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00461

Otheguy, Ricardo & Ana Celia Zentella. 2012. Spanish in New York. New York: Oxford University Press.

Paradis, Michel. 2004. A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.18

Pascual y Cabo, Diego. 2013. Agreement reflexes of emerging optionality in heritage speaker Spanish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Florida.

Pérez-Cortés, Silvia, Michael Putnam & Liliana Sánchez. 2019. Differential access: asymmetries in accessing features and building representations in heritage language grammars. Languages 4. 81. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3390/languages4040081

Pires, Acrisio & Jason Rothman. 2009. Disentangling contributing variables to incomplete acquisition competence outcomes: What differences across Brazilian and European Portuguese heritage speakers tell us. International Journal of Bilingualism 13. 211–238. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006909339806

Polinsky, Maria. 2018. Heritage languages and their speakers. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/9781107252349

Pomino, Natascha, Katrin Schmitz & Kathrin Anne Neuburger. 2018. Differential object marking in Spanish as a heritage language in Germany. Arbeitspapier 129. 1.

Prévost, Philippe & Lydia White. 2000. Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research 16. 110–133. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1191/026765800677556046

Putnam, Michael & Liliana Sánchez. 2013. What’s so incomplete about incomplete acquisition?: A prolegomenon to modeling heritage language grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 3. 478–508. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.3.4.04put

Putnam, Michael, Silvia Pérez-Cortés & Liliana Sánchez. 2019. Language attrition and the Feature Reassembly Hypothesis. In Barbara Köpke & Monika Schmid (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition, 18–24. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198793595.013.3

Rinke, Esther, Cristina Flores & Pilar Barbosa. 2018. Null objects in the spontaneous speech of monolingual and bilingual speakers of European Portuguese. Probus 30. 93–119. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/probus-2017-0004

Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, Miguel. 2008. The acquisition of differential object marking in Spanish. Probus 20. 111–145. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/PROBUS.2008.004

Rodríguez-Ordóñez, Itxaso. 2017. Reexamining differential object marking as a linguistic contact phenomenon in Gernika Basque. Journal of Language Contact 10. 318–352. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1163/19552629-01002004

Rothman, Jason. 2007. Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. The International Journal of Bilingualism 11. 359–389. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/13670069070110040201

Scontras, Gregory, Maria Polinsky & Zuzanna Fuchs. 2018. In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3(1): 1. 1–29. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.164

Silverstein, Michael. 1976. Hierarchy of features sand ergativity. In Robert Dixon (ed.), Grammatical Categories in Australian Languages, 121–171. Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press.

Sorace, Antonella. 2004. Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax-discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7. 143–145. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728904001543

Sorace, Antonella. 2011. Pinning down the concept of ‘interface’ in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1. 1–33. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.1.01sor

Sorace, Antonella. 2020. L1 attrition in a wider perspective. Second Language Research 36. 203–207. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0267658319895571

Ticio, Emma. 2015. Differential object marking in Spanish-English early bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 5. 62–90. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.5.1.03tic

Ticio, Emma & Larisa Avram. 2015. The acquisition of differential object marking in Spanish and Romanian: Semantic scales or semantic features? Revue Roumaine de Linguistique LX, 4. 383–402. Bucureşti.

Țigău, Alina. 2010. Towards an account of DOM in Romanian, Bucharest Working Papers in Linguistics 12(1). 137–158.

Țigău, Alina. 2011. Syntax and interpretation of the direct object in Romance and Germanic languages with an emphasis on Romanian, German, Dutch and English. Editura UniversităȚii din Bucureşti.

Torres, Julio. 2013. Heritage and second language learners of Spanish: The roles of task complexity and inhibitory control. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgetown University.

von Heusinger, Klaus & Edgar Onea Gáspár. 2008. Triggering and blocking effects in the diachronic development of DOM in Romanian. Probus 20. 67–110. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/PROBUS.2008.003

von Heusinger, Klaus & Georg Kaiser. 2005. The evolution of differential object marking in Spanish. In Klaus von Heusinger, Georg Kaiser & Elisabeth Stark (eds.), Proceedings of the Workshop “Specificity and the evolution/emergence of nominal determination in Romance.” Arbeitspapier 119. 33–69. Konstanz: Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft der Universitaet Konstanz.

von Heusinger, Klaus & Sofiana Chiriacescu. 2009. Definite “bare” nouns and pe-marking in Romanian. In María Teresa Espinal, Manuel Leonetti & Louise McNally (eds.), Proceedings of the IV Nereus International Workshop on “Definiteness and DP Structure in Romance Languages.” Arbeitspapier 124. 63–82. Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft, Universität Konstanz.

Yager, Lisa, Nora Hellmold, Hyoun-A Joo, Michael Putnam, Eleonora Rossi, Catherine Stafford, & Joseph Salmons. 2015. New structural patterns in moribund grammar: Case marking in heritage German. Frontiers in Psychology 6. 1716. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01716

Yeni-Komshian, Grace, James Flege & Serena Liu. 2000. Pronunciation proficiency in the first and second languages of Korean-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3. 131–149. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728900000225