1 Introduction

This paper discusses A-bar movement of possessors in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese, three Austronesian languages spoken primarily in parts of western Indonesia. These languages allow a possessor to occur external to its possessive DP, as in (1–3):1

    1. (1)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Siapa
    2. who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. baca
    2. read
    1. buku-nya?
    2. book-D
    1. ‘Who is it that little brother is reading (her) book?’
    1. (2)
    1. Javanese
    1. wong
    2. person
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru
    2. blue
    1. ‘the person whose book is blue’
    1. (3)
    1. Madurese
    1. Sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’?
    2. younger.sibling
    1. ‘Whose book was read by little brother?’

The possessor may occur as a clefted element as in (1) and (3), or as the head of a relative clause as in (2). In both cases the possessor occurs at the left periphery in an A-bar position, separated from the rest of the clause by a relative morpheme, while the possessum occurs in an A position.

The first aim of the paper is to show that the relevant structures have been derived by movement of the possessor, rather than base generation of the possessor in its surface position. The argument is based on a comparison of the three languages, which are closely related. Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese are sometimes described as “Indonesian-type languages” because of similarities in their morphosyntax, particularly their voice systems (in contrast with Philippine-type languages, e.g. see discussion in Arka 2002; Himmelmann 2002; Cole et al. 2008; Blust 2013). Despite the close parallels in syntactic structure, I show that the pattern of nominal extraction in familiar Madurese diverges from that of polite Madurese, Indonesian and Javanese. This difference has consequences for the ability of a possessor to move to the left periphery in familiar Madurese, which is explained only if external possession is derived by the same syntactic mechanisms as nominal A-bar extraction. I show that external possession is subject to the same constraints that apply to the movement of verbal arguments: there is a correlation between the ability of a subject or object to extract and the ability of an external possessor to be associated with the same argument position. Furthermore, for the languages that allow object extraction, verbal morphology obligatorily marks the extraction; the same pattern applies when an external possessor is associated with that object. This is taken as evidence that possessors undergo A-bar extraction in Indonesian, Javanese and polite Madurese.

Second, while extraction of verbal arguments has been well studied in Indonesian-type languages, possessor extraction contributes a novel view on A-bar movement. Possessor extraction provides support for successive cyclic movement through the edge of VoiceP for all nominals. In the analysis proposed here for possessor extraction in these languages, one of the functions of the head Voice is to regulate the movement of any nominal from the complement of V. This also sheds new light on the nature of “subject-only” extraction in these languages, because it is the ability to escape from VoiceP, rather than a structural subject position, that determines the availability of extraction. The conclusion is that nominal A-bar movement is regulated low in the clause, by the functional head Voice.

The paper is organized as follows. Section 2 provides background on possessor extraction and nominal movement in these languages. In Section 3 I present possessor extraction data in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese, showing that a movement analysis captures the patterns of extraction and verbal morphology. Section 4 discusses possessor movement through the edge of DP, then through the edge of VoiceP, which is driven by edge features on the functional heads D and Voice. Implications for Indonesian-type languages are discussed in Section 5. Conclusions are presented in Section 6, along with with remaining issues for future research.

2 Background

2.1 Preliminaries

My consultants speak varieties of Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese associated with East Java. A large portion of the data in this paper was collected during fieldwork with speakers from the cities of Surabaya and Malang. Data from spoken, colloquial varieties of each language are presented, unless otherwise noted. In addition to data from the familiar speech level of Madurese, I also present data from the polite speech level, which is a formal register but is primarily spoken rather than written.

External possession clauses occur infrequently, using a structure that can be interpreted as either a relative or a (pseudo-)cleft. Note that many of the examples therefore have two possible readings, corresponding to a relative clause or a clefted interpretation. For instance, (4) can either mean ‘Adi’s house that was destroyed yesterday’ (relative reading) or ‘It was Adi’s house that was destroyed yesterday’ (clefted reading).

    1. (4)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Rumah
    2. house
    1. Adi
    2. Adi
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1. ‘It was Adi’s house that was destroyed yesterday.’
    2. ‘Adi’s house that was destroyed yesterday.’

The clefted reading requires a certain information-structural context: information within in the relative clause must be presupposed. In speech, intonation as well as context disambiguate whether a cleft or relative is intended; the relative may also be embedded with a clause, as expected. The glosses used throughout the paper do not necessarily indicate preference for a particular reading.

2.2 Possessor extraction and external possession

External possession is an umbrella term for constructions in which a possessor occurs outside of the constituent with which it has a possession relation. In a recent overview, Deal (2013) identifies two broad categories of external possession (sometimes called possessor raising). The first has properties similar to raising and control, occurring in infinitivals in complement position; these cases are attested in numerous languages (see e.g. Keenan 1972; Perlmutter & Postal 1983; Baker 1988; Landau 1999; Payne & Barshi 1999; among many others). The second type of external possession involves only subjects, and triggers information-structural effects; this type is found in languages such as Chickasaw, dialects of Flemish, Japanese, Korean and Tz’utujil.

Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese possessors that occur at the left edge of the clause, as illustrated in (1–3), appear to be distinct from the range of external possession intended to be included in Deal’s typology. The possessors considered here are not limited to infinitivals and complement positions, but instead occur as the head of a pseudo-cleft or relative. Neither is the external possessor limited to subjects; external possession from object position is possible, as seen in (1). External possession can include either a wh possessor or a lexical possessor, and is not sensitive to inalienability, affectedness, part-whole relations or other conditions frequently associated with external possession. I take possessor extraction to be separate from phenomena that are generally labeled external possession or possessor raising.

However, it is worth mentioning that at least in Indonesian, some verbs allow an external possessor to occur in an A position:

    1. (5)
    1. Indonesian (Sneddon 1996: 279)
    1. Jakarta
    2. Jakarta
    1. sudah
    2. PERF
    1. mulai
    2. begin
    1. ber-ubah
    2. INTR-change
    1. wajah-nya.
    2. face-D
    1. ‘The face of Jakarta has begun to change.’
    1. (6)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Penyanyi
    2. singer
    1. itu
    2. that
    1. me-rasa
    2. ACTV-feel
    1. ter-ancam
    2. INVOL-threaten
    1. nyawa-nya.
    2. soul-D
    1. ‘The singer’s life felt threatened.’

These potential cases of possessor raising are not included in this discussion, as my goal is to build a case for possessors that undergo A-bar extraction directly from possessive DPs, particularly in transitive active sentences. I identify cases of A-bar extraction by the occurrence of a relative morpheme separating the possessor from the rest of the clause as in (1–3); I assume that the external possession in (5–6) is derived by another process.2 I leave examples such as (5–6) for future investigation.

While possessor extraction has been noted in other languages, (e.g. Ross 1986; Szabolcsi 1992; Gavruseva 2000; Coon 2009) it has not been widely discussed for languages related to Indonesian, Javanese or Madurese. Indonesian examples are briefly discussed in Chung (2008) and in grammars such as Sneddon (1996); Sneddon et al. (2012). External possessors in Madurese are mentioned in Davies (2003; 2010). Other Austronesian languages that have been noted to employ a similar cleft structure with an external possessor include Cebuano (Bell 1983) and Tagalog (Kroeger 1993).

2.3 Nominal movement in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese

Before discussing possessor extraction, I review general extraction of nominals in these three languages, which have the properties of A-bar movement (or WH movement in the sense of Chomsky 1977 and subsequent). In Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese, a nominal that has undergone A-bar movement occurs at the left edge of the clause, and must be immediately followed by a relative morpheme:

    1. (7)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Susan
    2. Susan
    1. tahu
    2. know
    1. Lani
    2. Lani
    1. suka
    2. like
    1. kue.
    2. cake
    1. ‘Susan knows Lani likes cake.’
    1. (8)
    1. Siapa
    2. who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. Susan
    2. Susan
    1. tahu
    2. know
    1. suka
    2. like
    1. kue?
    2. cake
    1. ‘Who does Susan know likes cake?’
    1. (9)
    1. Javanese
    1. Lina
    2. Lina
    1. senengi
    2. like
    1. kue.
    2. cake
    1. ‘Lina likes cake.’
    1. (10)
    1. kue
    2. cake
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. Lina
    2. Lina
    1. senengi
    2. like
    1. ‘the cake that Lina likes’
    1. (11)
    1. Madurese
    1. Adi
    2. Adi
    1. ng-akan
    2. ACTV-eat
    1. mie.
    2. noodles
    1. ‘Adi is eating noodles.’
    1. (12)
    1. Sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. ng-akan
    2. ACTV-eat
    1. mie?
    2. noodles
    1. ‘Who is eating noodles?’

A-bar movement in each of these examples results in a gap, for both relatives (10) and moved-wh questions (8, 12) (in-situ wh questions are also possible in all three languages). While nominal extraction can be long-distance in Indonesian (8), Davies (2000; 2003) shows that long-distance movement is impossible in Javanese and Madurese. For Javanese and Madurese, nominal extraction is only possible out of a single clause for questions and relatives.

Nominal A-bar movement in all three languages is also sensitive to islands. It is not possible to extract out of a complex NP (13–14) or adjunct clause (15–16) in Indonesian:

    1. (13)
    1.    Indonesian
    1.   Susan
    2.   Susan
    1. dapat
    2. get
    1. kesimpulan
    2. conclusion
    1. bahwa
    2. that
    1. Lani
    2. Lani
    1. suka
    2. like
    1. kue.
    2. cake
    1.    ‘Susan got the conclusion that Lani likes cake.’
    1. (14)
    1. *Siapa
    2.   who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. Susan
    2. Susan
    1. dapat
    2. get
    1. kesimpulan
    2. conclusion
    1. suka
    2. like
    1. kue?
    2. cake
    1.    (‘Who did Susan get the conclusion likes cake?’)
    1. (15)
    1.   Ayah
    2.   Father
    1. senang
    2. happy
    1. ketika
    2. when
    1. me-lihat
    2. ACTV-see
    1. film
    2. film
    1. itu.
    2. that
    1.    ‘Father was happy when he saw that film.’
    1. (16)
    1. *Apa
    2.   what
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. Ayah
    2. Father
    1. senang
    2. happy
    1. ketika
    2. when
    1. lihat?
    2. see
    1.    (‘What was Father happy when he saw?’)

Extraction out of a complex NP or adjunct clause is likewise impossible in Javanese and Madurese, but such movement would also be ruled out by the restriction on long distance movement in these languages. All three languages also disallow fronted PPs to occur with the relative morpheme (yang/sing/se), which is obligatory in cases of A-bar extraction. This is shown in the examples below, where PPs cannot occur with the relative morpheme, whether the preposition is stranded or pied-piped:

    1. (17)
    1.    Indonesian
    1.   Aku
    2.   1SG
    1. mau
    2. want
    1. bertemu
    2. meet
    1. dengan
    2. with
    1. Ibu
    2. Mrs
    1. guru.
    2. teacher
    1.    ‘I will meet with the teacher.’
    1. (18)
    1. *Siapa
    2.   who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. aku
    2. 1SG
    1. mau
    2. want
    1. bertemu
    2. meet
    1. dengan?
    2. with
    1.    (‘Who will I meet with?’)
    1. (19)
    1. *Dengan
    2.   with
    1. siapa
    2. who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. aku
    2. 1SG
    1. mau
    2. want
    1. bertemu?
    2. meet
    1.    (‘With whom will I meet?’)
    1. (20)
    1.    Javanese
    1.   Sepeda motor-e
    2.   motorbike-D
    1. Rini
    2. Rini
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. di-dol
    2. PASS-sell
    1. karo
    2. by
    1. adik.
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    ‘Rini’s motorbike will be sold by little brother.’
    1. (21)
    1. *Sopo
    2.   who
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. sepeda motor-e
    2. motorbike-D
    1. Rini
    2. Rini
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. di-dol
    2. PASS-sell
    1. karo?
    2. by
    1.    (‘Who will Rini’s motorbike be sold by?’)
    1. (22)
    1. *Karo
    2.   by
    1. sopo
    2. who
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. sepeda motor-e
    2. motorbike-D
    1. Rini
    2. Rini
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. di-dol?
    2. PASS-sell
    1.    (‘By whom will Rini’s motorbike be sold?’)
    1. (23)
    1.    Madurese
    1.   Sara
    2.   Sara
    1. mole
    2. go.home
    1. darih
    2. from
    1. roma-nah
    2. house-D
    1. Fatima.
    2. Fatima
    1.    ‘Sara went home from Fatima’s house.’
    1. (24)
    1. *Roma-nah
    2.   house-D
    1. sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. Sara
    2. Sara
    1. mole
    2. go.home
    1. darih?
    2. from
    1.    (‘Whose house did Sara go home from?’)
    1. (25)
    1. *Darih
    2.   from
    1. roma-nah
    2. house-D
    1. sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. Sara
    2. Sara
    1. mole?
    2. go.home
    1.    (‘From whose house did Sara go home?’)

Some PPs and other adjuncts may occur in sentence-initial position without the relative morpheme, but I take such cases to be scrambling or base generation rather than A-bar movement. In addition to these properties of A-bar movement, in Section 3 I discuss another diagnostic for A-bar movement that is specific to these languages, which is active verbal morphology that is sensitive to movement.

I have already mentioned that wh-questions, pseudo-clefts and relatives have the same surface structure in these languages.3 Various authors have examined pseudo-clefts in Indonesian and related languages. See Kader 1976; Cole & Hermon 1998; Cole, Hermon & Aman 1999; Kroeger 2009 for Malay and Indonesian; for discussion in other Austronesian languages see Paul 2001; Pearson 2001; Davies 2003; Massam 2003; Aldridge 2014. Following previous analyses, I assume that pseudo-clefts involve operator movement, rather than movement of the nominal that is pronounced as the head of the relative or cleft. The null operator undergoes relative-internal movement to form a headless relative clause that is nominalized (e.g. ‘the one reading this book’ in (26)). This headless relative begins as the grammatical subject of a copular sentence (these languages have null copula). The predicate of this copular sentence is the nominal that is pronounced as the head of the cleft, for example ‘Siti’ in (26). In the base word order of pseudo-clefts, the headless relative clause occurs first, followed by the clefted nominal. This is shown in (26), (28) and (30):

    1. (26)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Yang
    2. REL
    1. baca
    2. read
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. ini
    2. this
    1. Siti.
    2. who
    1. ‘The one reading this book is Siti.’
    1. (27)
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. baca
    2. read
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. ini.
    2. this
    1. ‘It is Siti who is reading this book.’
    1. (28)
    1. Javanese
    1. Sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru
    2. blue
    1. sopo?
    2. who
    1. ‘The one whose book is blue, is who?’
    1. (29)
    1. Sopo
    2. who
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru?
    2. blue
    1. ‘Whose book is blue?’
    1. (30)
    1. Madurese
    1. Se
    2. REL
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’
    2. younger.sibling
    1. apah?
    2. what
    1. ‘The thing that was read by little brother, is what?’
    1. (31)
    1. Apah
    2. what
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’?
    2. younger.sibling
    1. ‘What was read by little brother?’

The pseudo-cleft is derived by raising the predicate nominal to a sentence-initial focused position, resulting in the surface word order in (27), (29) and (31). Although the predicate nominal frequently occurs at the left edge of the clause, the examples above show that it may occur in either sentence-initial position or sentence-final position.4

I treat null operator movement as the strategy used in all cases of extraction (wh constituent questions, relatives and pseudo-clefts). In other words, I assume that the overt nominal does not undergo A-bar movement itself. It is always the null operator which undergoes movement within a relative clause, and I assume that this operator is subject to constraints on nominal movement, such as syntactic islands. Note that others authors have also treated null operator movement as the strategy for A-bar/WH movement in various languages, e.g. Chung 1998; McCloskey 2001; 2002; Reintges et al. 2006. For ease of exposition however, in this paper I refer to “nominal movement,” “possessor extraction,” etc. rather than “null operator movement” or “operator extraction.”

3 Possessor sub-extraction in three Indonesian-type languages

In this section I demonstrate that the external possessors in (1–3) are not base generated in their surface positions at the left edge of the clause. Rather, the possessor is extracted from its possessive DP to an A-bar position, while the possessum remains in situ, in an A position. Two primary pieces of evidence support the claim. First, across these three languages, the ability of the subject or object to be extracted predicts whether an external possessor can be associated with that argument. Second, the voice morphology that is correlated with nominal extraction in these languages is also sensitive to external possessors.

3.1 Extraction from subject position

I begin with possessive DPs that occur in the preverbal position of grammatical subjects. For each language, I show an example of internal possession, in which the possessor occurs within a possessive DP in subject position. The second example shows that the possessive DP (both possessum and possessor) may be extracted from subject position to the left edge of the clause, obligatorily followed by a relative morpheme (Indonesian yang, Javanese sing, Madurese se). Third, an example of an external possessor is given, where the possessor occurs at the left edge of the clause while the possessum remains in-situ as subject.

    1. (32)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Rumah
    2. house
    1. Adi
    2. Adi
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1. ‘Adi’s house was destroyed yesterday.’
    1. (33)
    1. Rumah
    2. house
    1. Adi
    2. Adi
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1. ‘It was Adi’s house that was destroyed yesterday.’
    1. (34)
    1. orang
    2. person
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. rumah-nya
    2. house-D
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin
    2. yesterday
    1. ‘the person whose house was destroyed yesterday’
    1. (35)
    1. Javanese
    1. Buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. Rini
    2. Rini
    1. di-woco
    2. PASS-read
    1. adik.
    2. younger.sibling
    1. ‘Rini’s book was read by little brother.’
    1. (36)
    1. Buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. Rini
    2. Rini
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. di-woco
    2. PASS-read
    1. adik.
    2. younger.sibling
    1. ‘It was Rini’s book that was read by little brother.’
    1. (37)
    1. Sopo
    2. who
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. di-woco
    2. PASS-read
    1. adik?
    2. younger.sibling
    1. ‘Who is it that (her) book was read by little brother?’
    1. (38)
    1. Madurese (familiar)
    1. Kalambhi-nah
    2. clothing-D
    1. Joko
    2. Joko
    1. e-sasa.
    2. PASS-wash
    1. ‘Joko’s clothing was washed.’
    1. (39)
    1. Kalambhi-nah
    2. clothing-D
    1. Joko
    2. Joko
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. e-sasa.
    2. PASS-wash
    1. ‘It was Joko’s clothing that was washed.’
    1. (40)
    1. Sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. kalambhi-nah
    2. clothing-D
    1. e-sasa?
    2. PASS-wash
    1. ‘Who is it that (his) clothing was washed?’

Subjects are freely extracted in these languages, and (34), (37) and (40) show that an external possessor can be associated with a possessum in subject position. This is the case whether the external possessor is a wh word or lexical possessor.5 Subject extraction is not dependent on the thematic role of the nominal: (32–40) are passive clauses in which the Theme occurs in subject position, but subject Agents in active clauses behave similarly, as shown in (52–53).

One characteristic of the grammatical subject in these languages is that it must be definite or specific (Cole et al. 2002; Sato 2008; Davies 2010; Sneddon et al. 2012). Even though these languages typically allow in situ questions, with a wh phrase occurring in object position or as a prepositional object, since wh words are non-specific, an in situ subject question is disallowed.67 Interestingly, if the subject is a possessive DP, it is the definiteness of the possessor, rather than the definiteness of the possessum, that fulfills the subject requirement. This is illustrated below in Madurese. In contrast to the definite possessor Joko in (38), the generic noun oreng ‘person’ cannot occur as a possessor in subject position (41). The subject question in (42) is also not possible because the wh possessor renders the subject non-specific.

    1. (41)
    1.    Madurese (familiar)
    1. *Kalambhi-nah
    2.   clothing-D
    1. oreng
    2. person
    1. e-sasa.
    2. PASS-wash
    1.    (‘Somebody’s clothing was washed.’)
    1. (42)
    1. *Kalambhi-nah
    2.   clothing-D
    1. sapah
    2. who
    1. e-sasa?
    2. PASS-wash
    1.    (‘Whose clothing was washed?’)
    1. (43)
    1. Kalambhi-nah
    2. clothing-D
    1. sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. e-sasa?
    2. PASS-wash
    1. ‘Whose clothing is it that was washed?’

Instead, the entire subject may be clefted (43), or the external possessor can be clefted as previously shown in (40). Similar observations apply in Indonesian and Javanese:

    1. (44)
    1.    Indonesian
    1. *Rumah
    2.   house
    1. orang
    2. person
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1.    (‘Somebody’s house was destroyed yesterday.’)
    1. (45)
    1.    Javanese
    1. *Buku-ne
    2.   book-D
    1. sopo
    2. who
    1. di-woco
    2. PASS-read
    1. adik?
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    (‘Whose book was read by little brother?’)

The definiteness requirement in these languages means that wh possessors in subject position cannot be left in-situ.

3.2 Extraction from object position

Next, I turn to possessive DPs in postverbal object position. I demonstrate that the extraction facts are similar for Indonesian and Javanese, whereas the pattern deviates in familiar Madurese. Further investigation shows that the polite register of Madurese patterns with Indonesian and Javanese, rather than familiar Madurese.

Both Indonesian and Javanese allow extraction of an internal argument in an active transitive clause (47, 50). An external possessor may also be associated with a possessum in object position (48, 51).

    1. (46)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. mem-baca
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. gadis
    2. girl
    1. itu.8
    2. that
    1. ‘Little brother is reading the girl’s book.’
    1. (47)
    1. Buku
    2. book
    1. gadis
    2. girl
    1. itu
    2. that
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. baca/
    2. read/
    1. *mem-baca.
    2.   ACTV-read
    1. ‘It is the girl’s book that little brother is reading.’
    1. (48)
    1. Siapa
    2. who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. baca/
    2. read/
    1. *mem-baca
    2.   ACTV-read
    1. buku-nya?
    2. book-D
    1. ‘Who is it that little brother is reading (her) book?’
    1. (49)
    1. Javanese
    1. Aku
    2. 1SG
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. m-oco
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku-ne
    2. buku-D
    1. penulis
    2. writer
    1. iku.
    2. that
    1. ‘I will read that writer’s book.’
    1. (50)
    1. Buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. penulis
    2. writer
    1. iku
    2. that
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. aku
    2. 1SG
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. woco/
    2. read/
    1. *m-oco.
    2.   ACTV-read
    1. ‘It is that writer’s book that I will read.’
    1. (51)
    1. penulis
    2. writer
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. aku
    2. 1SG
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. woco/
    2. read/
    1. *m-oco
    2.   ACTV-read
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. ‘the writer that I will read (her) book’

When the internal argument is extracted in (47) and (50), the active voice prefix (Indonesian meN-, Javanese N-) cannot occur on the verb. Instead, a “bare verb” (i.e. no active voice morphology) is required for object extraction. Crucially, a bare verb is also required when an external possessor is associated with the object in (48) and (51). This contrasts with subject extraction from the same clauses, which are well-formed with the AV prefix:

    1. (52)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. mem-baca
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. gadis
    2. girl
    1. itu.
    2. that
    1. ‘It is little brother who is reading the girl’s book.’
    1. (53)
    1. Javanese
    1. Aku
    2. 1SG
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. kate
    2. will
    1. m-oco
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku-ne
    2. buku-D
    1. penulis
    2. writer
    1. iku.
    2. that
    1. ‘It is I who will read that writer’s book.’

Additionally, in-situ object wh questions are compatible with AV morphology on the verb:

    1. (54)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. mem-baca
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. siapa?
    2. who
    1. ‘Whose book is little brother reading?’
    1. (55)
    1. Javanese
    1. Aku
    2. 1SG
    1. m-oco
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku-ne
    2. buku-D
    1. sopo?
    2. who
    1. ‘Whose book was I reading?’

The bare verb, then, is obligatory only when the extraction occurs from object position.

The correlation between object extraction and null voice morphology in active clauses has been well discussed in the literature for varieties of Indonesian and related languages (e.g. Saddy 1991; Voskuil 1996; 2000; Cole & Hermon 2005; Aldridge 2008; Arka & Manning 2008; Cole et al. 2008; Fortin 2009; Davies 2010; Yanti 2010; Sato 2012; among others). The data presented here show that the pattern also applies to a set of overlooked nominals: possessors. In active transitive clauses in Indonesian and Javanese, a bare verb is obligatory with object extraction, but not subject extraction; a bare verb is also obligatory when the external possessor is associated with the object, but not when associated with the subject.

Familiar Madurese provides a contrast with Indonesian and Javanese; the internal argument of an active transitive clause cannot be extracted (Davies 2010; Jeoung 2017).

    1. (56)
    1.    Madurese (familiar)
    1.   Ale’
    2.   younger.sibling
    1. m-acah
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. Tono.
    2. Tono
    1.    ‘Little brother read Tono’s book.’
    1. (57)
    1. *Buku-nah
    2.   book-D
    1. Tono
    2. Tono
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. ale’
    2. younger.sibling
    1. m-acah/bacah.
    2. ACTV-read/read
    1.    (‘It was Tono’s book that little brother read.’)
    1. (58)
    1. *Sapah
    2.   who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. ale’
    2. younger.sibling
    1. m-acah/bacah
    2. ACTV-read/read
    1. buku-nah?
    2. book-D
    1.    (‘Who was it that little brother read his book?’)

(57) illustrates that object extraction in familiar Madurese is not possible, whether the verb is bare or has the active prefix. Likewise, an external possessor cannot be associated with a possessum in object position in (58), whether the verb is bare or affixed. We have already seen that in familiar Madurese, it is possible to extraction the Theme from subject position in a passive clause (39); the subject (external argument) of an active transitive clause may also be extracted, and does not require a bare verb:

    1. (59)
    1. Madurese (familiar)
    1. Ale’
    2. younger.sibling
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. m-acah
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. Tono.
    2. Tono
    1. ‘It was little brother who read Tono’s book.’

Familiar Madurese therefore diverges from the pattern seen Indonesian and Javanese. While subject extraction is licit in familiar Madurese, object extraction is not possible; and external possessors can only be associated with subjects, but not objects.

The availability of extraction, then, is correlated with the availability of external possession across Indonesian, Javanese and familiar Madurese: if a subject or object can be extracted from a certain position, then an external possessor may be associated with a possessum in that position. I take these facts to indicate that external possessors must undergo the same type of movement as verbal arguments, such that the mechanisms that allow or prevent the extraction of a verbal argument will also apply to its possessor. This is further supported by additional observations from the polite register of Madurese.

An unusual fact about Madurese is that the familiar and polite registers behave differently with regard to argument extraction (Jeoung 2017). The familiar register allows only subject extraction, but the polite register allows a possessive DP to be extracted from both subject position (61) and object position (63).

    1. (60)
    1. Madurese (polite)
    1. Buku-epon
    2. book-D
    1. pak
    2. Mr
    1. ustadz
    2. teacher
    1. e-maos
    2. PASS-read
    1. sareng
    2. by
    1. rajih-epon.
    2. wife-D
    1. ‘Teacher’s book was read by his wife.’
    1. (61)
    1. Buku-epon
    2. book-D
    1. pak
    2. Mr
    1. ustadz
    2. teacher
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. e-maos
    2. PASS-read
    1. sareng
    2. by
    1. rajih-epon.
    2. wife-D
    1. ‘It was Teacher’s book that was read by his wife.’
    1. (62)
    1. Kaulah
    2. 1SG
    1. lastareh
    2. PERF
    1. m-acah
    2. ACTV-read
    1. buku-epon
    2. book-D
    1. imam
    2. imam
    1. ka’dissah.
    2. that
    1. ‘I already read that imam’s book.’9
    1. (63)
    1. Buku-epon
    2. book-D
    1. imam
    2. imam
    1. ka’dissah
    2. that
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. kaulah
    2. 1SG
    1. lastareh
    2. PERF
    1. bacah/
    2. read/
    1. *m-acah.
    2.   ACTV-read
    1. ‘It was that imam’s book that I already read.’

Polite Madurese therefore displays the same pattern as Indonesian and Javanese, which allow both subject and object extraction.10 Also consistent with this pattern, object extraction in polite Madurese disallows the active prefix on the verb (63).

If possessors undergo A-bar extraction as verbal arguments do, this predicts that polite Madurese will pattern with Indonesian and Javanese in allowing an external possessor to be associated with a possessum in either subject or object position. This is the case:

    1. (64)
    1. Madurese (polite)
    1. Pak
    2. Mr
    1. ustadz
    2. teacher
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. buku-epon
    2. book-D
    1. e-maos
    2. PASS-read
    1. sareng
    2. by
    1. rajih-epon.
    2. wife-D
    1. ‘It was Teacher that (his) book was read by his wife.’
    1. (65)
    1. Imam
    2. imam
    1. ka’dissah
    2. that
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. kaulah
    2. 1SG
    1. lastareh
    2. PERF
    1. bacah/
    2. read/
    1. *m-acah
    2.   ACTV-read
    1. buku-epon.
    2. book-D
    1. ‘It was that imam that I already read (his) book.’

Also predicted is the fact that the verbal morphology is required to be bare when the external possessor is associated with the object in (65).

To summarize this section, the ability of a subject or object to be extracted is correlated with the ability of an external possessor to be associated with that argument position. I take this as evidence that the possessor undergoes A-bar extraction from an argument position to its surface position at the left periphery of the clause. An alternate analysis, in which the possessor is base-generated in its surface position, does not account for the subject-object asymmetry in verbal morphology; it is difficult to explain why a high base-generated possessor requires a null active voice prefix when the possessum is the object, but not when the possessum is the grammatical subject. By contrast, the pattern is easily explained under the present analysis. A-bar movement of a possessor is subject to the same constraints as A-bar movement of an argument. Given that subjects may be freely extracted in Indonesian, Javanese and polite Madurese, but objects may be extracted only with null active voice morphology, it follows that the possessors of these arguments show the same pattern. In a similar vein, subjects and their possessors may be extracted in familiar Madurese, but objects and their possessors may not. This is consistent with much previous work in Indonesian languages that observes obligatory null verbal morphology as indicative of A-bar movement of the internal object (Saddy 1991; Voskuil 1996; 2000; Cole & Hermon 2005; Aldridge 2008; Arka & Manning 2008; Cole et al. 2008; Sato 2008; Fortin 2009; Davies 2010; Yanti 2010; Sato 2012; Jeoung 2017; among others).

3.3 The suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah in internal possession

In this section I discuss the status of the suffix that occurs on the possessum: -nya in Indonesian, -ne or -e in Javanese, and -Nah in Madurese. For Indonesian, when this suffix occurs in internal possession, it is sometimes called a possessive linker or ligature (e.g. Sneddon 1996; Arka 2013), but with an extracted possessor, it has been assumed to be a resumptive pronoun (e.g. Voskuil 2000; Musgrave 2001; Chung 2008).

I propose an analysis in which the suffix that occurs in internal possession is the same as that which occurs in possessor extraction. This is straightforward in Javanese and Madurese, since the suffix consistently occurs with both internal and external possessors (with the exception of Javanese possessors that are clitic pronouns; see Table 1). In Indonesian, the suffix -nya is optionally pronounced in internal possession, but obligatorily pronounced when the possessor has been extracted. This analysis of -nya/-ne/-Nah is supported by the following evidence: first, the suffix and the definite morpheme in each language share the same distribution and identity in form, suggesting that they are both the functional head D; second, the suffix may co-occur with personal pronouns, names and other possessors in internal possession, which indicates that the suffix is not pronominal. I also argue that the possessor may be a pro argument, which derives the default 3 person interpretation of -nya/-ne/-Nah.

Table 1

Possessive DPs with pronominal possessors.

rumah ‘house’ + possessor
-D + possessor pronominal clitic ∅ + possessor
Indonesian11
1SG ‘my house’
2SG ‘your house’
3SG ‘his/her house’
pro possessor
cf. ‘Rika’s house’
cf. ‘whose house’
rumah-nya aku12
rumah-nya kamu
rumah-nya dia
rumah-nya pro
rumah-nya Rika
rumah-nya siapa
rumah-ku
rumah-mu



rumah aku
rumah kamu
rumah dia

rumah Rika
rumah siapa
Javanese13
1SG ‘my house’
2SG ‘your house’
3SG ‘his/her house’
pro possessor
cf. ‘Rika’s house’
cf. ‘whose house’
omah-ne aku14
omah-ne kamu
omah-ne dewe’e
omah-ne pro
omah-ne Rika
omah-ne sopo
omah-ku
omah-mu








Madurese
1SG ‘my house’
2SG ‘your house’
3SG ‘his/her house’
pro possessor
cf. ‘Rika’s house’
cf. ‘whose house’

roma-nah hedah
roma-nah aba’eng
roma-nah pro
roma-nah Rika
roma-nah sapah
tang kancah









To begin, these suffixes independently occur as definite morphemes in each language. Recall that subjects in these languages must be definite or specific. The suffixes in (66–68) mark the subject as definite, and the clause is ill-formed without it:

    1. (66)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Tempat-*(nya)
    2. place-D
    1. bagus
    2. good
    1. buat
    2. make
    1. foto~foto.
    2. photo~PL
    1. ‘The place was good for photos.’
    1. (67)
    1. Javanese
    1. Ali-ali
    2. ring
    1. emmas-*(e)
    2. gold-D
    1. ce’
    2. very
    1. larang-e.
    2. expensive-INTENS
    1. ‘The gold ring is very expensive.’
    1. (68)
    1. Madurese
    1. Ghuruh-*(nah)
    2. teacher-D
    1. penter.
    2. smart
    1. ‘The teacher is smart.’

These are non-possessive contexts; note that the form of the the suffixes that occur in possessive DPs is identical to the definite morphemes in (66–68). This identity in form extends to variable morphological alternations in Javanese: there is optionality between -ne and -e for many speakers, who accept or produce both forms suffixed to the same N, in both definite contexts and possessive contexts. For Madurese, the first segment of the suffix –Nah is phonologically conditioned by the preceding segment (Stevens 1968; Davies 2010); the same set of allomorphs occur as definite suffixes and possessive suffixes in Madurese. (The Indonesian suffix -nya is invariant.) This identity in form and distribution suggests that these suffixes are the functional head D; see Section 4.1 for trees showing possessive DP structure.

In all three languages, these suffixes co-occur with personal pronouns in possessive DPs. See Table 1, which shows the possessum ‘house’ with a representative set of pronominal possessors.15 Two observations are worth noting about the forms in Table 1. The first is that in Javanese and Madurese, the suffix is required in all possessive DPs (with the exception of clitic possessors in Javanese and the unusual 1 singular clitic tang in Madurese).1617 The possessum is suffixed with -ne/-Nah with both internal and external possessors. For Javanese and Madurese, then, there is little reason to believe that -ne/-Nah is a resumptive pronoun when the possessor has been extracted. Extraction of the possessor merely leaves the possessum and suffix in situ, as expected. This is also the analysis that I pursue for Indonesian below, although the multi-functionality of Indonesian -nya requires additional discussion.

The second point illustrated in Table 1 is that for Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese, there is no 3 possessive pronoun. This departs from the view in previous literature, which is that the suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah is a pronominal possessor, and that it has 3 person features (e.g. Sneddon 1996; Errington 1998; Davies 2010; Sneddon et al. 2012; Arka 2013). Instead of a 3 person pronominal analysis for -nya/-ne/-Nah, I propose a silent pro possessor, a reasonable assumption given that these are pro-drop languages and silent arguments are very common. Consider again the examples in (66–68), in which a noun is suffixed with -nya/-ne/-Nah. In non-possessive contexts, this is interpreted as a definite morpheme. However, if a possessor is salient in the discourse, these are interpreted as 3 person possessives (examples repeated with possessive glosses):

    1. (69)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Tempat-nah
    2. place-D
    1. pro
    2.  
    1. bagus
    2. good
    1. buat
    2. make
    1. foto~foto.
    2. photo~PL
    1. ‘Her place was good for photos.’
    1. (70)
    1. Javanese
    1. Ali-ali
    2. ring
    1. emmas-e
    2. gold-D
    1. pro
    2.  
    1. ce’
    2. very
    1. larang-e.
    2. expensive-INTENS
    1. ‘Her gold ring is very expensive.’
    1. (71)
    1. Madurese
    1. Ghuruh-nah
    2. teacher-D
    1. pro
    2.  
    1. penter.
    2. smart
    1. ‘His teacher is smart.’

The pro possessor explains why these possessive DPs are interpreted as 3 person; a pro argument receives a default 3 person interpretation. This proposal derives the same interpretive result as a 3 person pronominal analysis for -nya/-ne/-Nah, but has the advantage of simplicity, making the distribution of the suffix consistent across all forms in Table 1. The reason that -nya/-ne/-Nah is compatible with 1, 2 and 3 arguments is that it does not bear any phi features. Under this view, it is not necessary to stipulate that the suffix on the possessum is sometimes a linker/ligature, but sometimes also a 3 person possessor. This analysis also accounts for the ability of the suffix to co-occur with a 3 pronoun.

Let us turn specifically to Indonesian, which shows a different pattern than Javanese and Madurese: the suffix -nya is not required in internal possession, as shown in the last column of Table 1. Most authors describe possessive DPs in Standard Indonesian without -nya (e.g. Sneddon et al. 2012), although examples of -nya in possessive DPs are also reported (Arka 2013). I find that in colloquial Indonesian, the suffix can optionally occur between the possessum and possessor (except when the possessor is a clitic pronoun):

    1. (72)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Buku(-nya)
    2. book-D
    1. dia
    2. 3SG
    1. biru,
    2. blue
    1. kalau
    2. as.for
    1. buku(-nya)
    2. book-D
    1. Desy
    2. Desy
    1. kuning.
    2. yellow
    1. ‘His book is blue, but Desy’s book is yellow.’
    1. (73)
    1. Uang(-nya)
    2. money-D
    1. orang
    2. person
    1. kaya
    2. rich
    1. cepat
    2. quick
    1. di-keluar-kan.
    2. PASS-exit-APPL
    1. ‘Rich people’s money is quickly spent.’

In Indonesian, the variable pronunciation of -nya as in (72–73) is sometimes reported to be the result of contact with various local languages, or attributed to regional differences. However, for my Indonesian consultants, (72–73) are possible both with and without the suffix, with no semantic consequence. (Recall that the definiteness of a possessive DP depends on the definiteness of the possessor; pronunciation of -nya does not carry definite semantics.) Analogous examples in Javanese and Madurese demonstrate the parallel internal structure of these possessive DPs across all three languages:

    1. (74)
    1. Javanese
    1. Buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. dewe’e
    2. 3SG
    1. biru,
    2. blue
    1. lek
    2. as.for
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. Desy
    2. Desy
    1. kuning.
    2. yellow
    1. ‘His book is blue, but Desy’s book is yellow.’
    1. (75)
    1. Duwit-e
    2. money-D
    1. wong
    2. person
    1. sogih
    2. rich
    1. endang
    2. quick
    1. di-tokk-e.
    2. PASS-buy-APPL
    1. ‘Rich people’s money is quickly spent.’
    1. (76)
    1. Madurese (familiar)
    1. Buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. Adi
    2. Adi
    1. bhiruh,
    2. blue
    1. mon
    2. as.for
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. Desy
    2. Desy
    1. kuning.
    2. yellow.
    1. ‘Ali’s book is blue, but Desy’s book is yellow.’
    1. (77)
    1. Pesse-nah
    2. money-D
    1. oreng
    2. person
    1. soghi
    2. rich
    1. dhulih
    2. quick
    1. e-pa-keluar.
    2. PASS-CAUS-exit
    1. ‘Rich people’s money is quickly spent.’

I conclude that Indonesian differs from Javanese and Madurese in that the suffix is optionally pronounced in internal possession. But like Javanese and Madurese, the suffix is obligatory with a pro possessor.

For the sake of completeness, I briefly note some other uses of Indonesian -nya that have been reported in the literature, without attempting to provide an analysis of all occurrences of this form. It is clear that -nya has different functions in contexts other than possession and definite marking, and these have received varied analyses by previous authors (see Musgrave 2001; Sneddon et al. 2012; Arka 2013; Kroeger 2014 for recent treatments of Indonesian -nya.) My purpose in outlining these cases is to demonstrate that a unified analysis for all cases of -nya is unlikely. The approach that I have pursued here is to show that specifically in cases of possession, -nya is the same suffix that marks definiteness on nominals. Other occurrences of -nya do not involve possession, but rather another function. For instance, -nya is used in the formation of evidential, modal and adverbial expressions (Arka 2013):18

    1. (78)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Ke-lihat-an-nya
    2. NMLZ-see-NMLZ-NYA
    1. Djoko
    2. Djoko
    1. sakit
    2. sick
    1. demam.
    2. fever
    1. ‘Apparently, Djoko was sick with fever.’
    1. (80)
    1. Pokok-nya
    2. primary-NYA
    1. kita
    2. 1SG.INCL
    1. saling
    2. each.other
    1. me-maham-i.
    2. ACTV-understand-APPL
    1. ‘Most importantly, we understand each other.’
    1. (79)
    1. Arka (2013: 35)
    1. Harus-nya
    2. must-NYA
    1. kamu
    2. 2SG
    1. datang.
    2. come
    1. ‘You should have come.’

-Nya can also attach to transitive verbs and prepositions:19

    1. (81)
    1. Indonesian
    1. Ibu
    2. Mother
    1. mem-per-indah
    2. ACTV-CAUS-beautiful
    1. baju-ku
    2. shirt-1SG
    1. dengan
    2. with
    1. men-jahit-(nya).
    2. ACTV-sew-NYA
    1. ‘Mother adorned my shirt by sewing it.’
    1. (82)
    1. Arka (2013: 34, with glosses modified)
    1. Perintah-ku
    2. order-2SG
    1. pada-nya
    2. to-NYA
    1. untuk
    2. for
    1. tak
    2. NEG
    1. kemana-mana.
    2. to.anywhere
    1. ‘My order to him is that (he should) not go anywhere.’

Unlike internal possession however, when -nya is cliticized to a transitive verb or a preposition, it cannot co-occur with another pronoun or nominal:

    1. (83)
    1.    Indonesian
    1. *Ibu
    2.   Mother
    1. mem-per-indah
    2. ACTV-CAUS-beautiful
    1. baju-ku
    2. shirt-1SG
    1. dengan
    2. with
    1. men-jahit-nya
    2. ACTV-sew-NYA
    1. itu/saku.20
    2. that/pocket
    1.    (‘Mother adorned my shirt by sewing it/a pocket.’)
    1. (84)
    1. *Perintah-ku
    2.   order-1SG
    1. pada-nya
    2. to-NYA
    1. dia
    2. 3SG
    1. untuk
    2. for
    1. tak
    2. NEG
    1. kemana-mana.
    2. to.anywhere
    1.    ‘My order to him is that (he should) not go anywhere.’

The suffix -nya can also occur with left-dislocated topics:

    1. (85)
    1. Indonesian (modified from Voskuil 2000: 207)
    1. Surat
    2. letter
    1. ini,
    2. this
    1. saya
    2. 1SG
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. me-nulis-(nya).
    2. ACTV-write-NYA
    1. ‘As for this letter, it is I who wrote it.’

In (85) -nya occurs in the position where a resumptive pronoun might be expected (i.e. the position of a trace or copy). Crucially however, (85) cannot be derived via A-bar movement: if the internal argument has been extracted, active voice morphology is disallowed on the verb. In addition, the topic surat ini does not occur with the relative morpheme yang, which is obligatory in all relatives and clefts. These properties cast doubt on a uniform analysis for possessive -nya and the other varied functions of -nya.

Returning to all three languages, I have argued against a pronominal analysis of the suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah as a 3 possessive form. The pronominal view must stipulate that the suffix is accidentally homophonous between a 3 possessive pronoun and another morpheme that occurs between the possessum and possessor. Having shown that -nya/-ne/-Nah is not a pronominal form in internal possession (cf. Table 1; examples (95–97)), a resumptive analysis for external possession is no longer supported. A resumptive view of -nya/-ne/-Nah fails to explain why 1 and 2 forms cannot occur resumptively in cases of possessor extraction. Furthermore, a resumptive analysis for -nya would predict that this form can be used resumptively for typical (non-possessive) DP extraction, which is not possible:

    1. (86)
    1.    Indonesian21
    1. *Apa
    2.   what
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. baca-nya?
    2. read-NYA
    1.    (‘What did little brother read?’)

Finally, another approach might be to consider -nya/-ne/-Nah a type of clitic doubling; cases of possessive doubling are attested in other languages. However, analogous doubling with 1 and 2 clitics is impossible; see examples (112–117). It is not clear why doubling would be limited to 3 person possessors.

In summary, the suffix -ne/-Nah is obligatory in Javanese and Madurese possession, whether the possessor is internal or external. In Indonesian, the suffix -nya is optionally pronounced with internal possessors (but not with clitic pronouns). In all three languages, the suffix is obligatory with a non-overt (pro) possessor, and when the possessor has been extracted. In the next section, I return to possessor extraction, and argue that the suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah is the head D.

4 Deriving possessor extraction

In this section, I argue that the external possessor undergoes movement through the edge of DP, then the edge of VoiceP, before landing in its surface position in the CP domain. This movement is driven by the edge feature [D] on D and Voice, which indicates that these are phase heads. A-bar movement of the possessor is morphologically marked for each phase: the functional head D must be pronounced as a suffix on the possessum, while the head Voice must be a null prefix on the verb. I argue that this special morphology is a type of wh-agreement.

4.1 Possessors escape possessive DPs through SpecDP

In the first step of extraction, a possessor must move through the edge of its possessive DP; from the specifier of DP, it is available for further movement. Szabolcsi (1992) proposes similar movement for Hungarian possessor extraction, observing that possessors cannot move directly from their base-generated positions, but must pass through the specifier of the noun phrase as an escape hatch. In a different approach, Gavruseva (2000) proposes that possessors in Hungarian, Tzotzil and Chamorro must first undergo DP-internal A-movement to AgrP in order to check uninterpretable phi features, followed by A-bar movement to SpecDP. I do not pursue Gavruseva’s argument for possessor movement through AgrP, which has little motivation in Indonesian-type languages. However, in the following analysis, the movement of the possessor is indeed driven by formal features in the syntax, and this movement has a morphological consequence: the obligatory pronunciation of D on the possessum.

For possessive DPs such as the Madurese example in (87), I assume the structure in (88) for all three languages:

    1. (87)
    1. Madurese
    1. padha-nah
    2. foot-D
    1. pak
    2. Mr
    1. Djoko
    2. Djoko
    1. ‘Mr Djoko’s foot’
    1. (88)
    1. Possessive DP structure

The possessor is merged in the specifier of NP (recall that the “possessor” can have a number of different semantic relations with the “possessum” in these languages, and is not limited to relations that are inalienable, part-whole etc.). The possessum undergoes head movement to D, so that the possessum is suffixed with -D and spelled out as the suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah. This derives the surface word order of possessive DPs, in which the possessum precedes the possessor.

The following examples in Indonesian provide evidence for the structure in (88). First consider a noun with a nominal complement in (89); the complement follows the head N. In a possessive construction, the possessum (head N) must undergo head movement to D. Examples (90–92) show that it is not possible for the complement to raise with the head N and precede the possessor (while retaining the meaning ‘book of children’s stories’).

    1. (89)
    1.    Indonesian
    1.   buku
    2.   book
    1. [cerita
    2. story
    1. anak]
    2. child
    1.    ‘book of children’s stories’
    1. (90)
    1. *buku
    2.   book
    1. [cerita
    2. story
    1. anak]
    2. child
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. (91)
    1. *buku
    2.   book
    1. [cerita
    2. story
    1. anak]
    2. child
    1. -nya
    2. -D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. (92)
    1. *buku-nya
    2.   book-D
    1. [cerita
    2. story
    1. anak]
    2. child
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. (93)
    1. *buku-nya
    2.   book-D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. [cerita
    2. story
    1. anak]
    2. child
    1. (94)
    1.   buku
    2.   book
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. [tentang
    2. about
    1. cerita
    2. story
    1. anak]
    2. child
    1.    ‘Siti’s book about children’s stories’

Interestingly, (93) shows that when N raises to D, the nominal complement cannot be stranded after the possessor. Instead, a periphrastic PP construction must be used (94). A similar pattern holds for PP adjuncts that modify the head noun, illustrated below in Javanese. The PP is usually postnominal (95), but cannot occur between possessum and possessor (96–97). Instead, the PP must occur outside the possessor (98).

    1. (95)
    1.    Javanese
    1.   murid
    2.   student
    1. [teko’
    2. from
    1. Jakarta]
    2. Jakarta
    1.    ‘a student from Jakarta’
    1. (96)
    1. *murid-e
    2.   student-D
    1. [teko’
    2. from
    1. Jakarta]
    2. Jakarta
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. (97)
    1. *murid
    2.   student
    1. [teko’
    2. from
    1. Jakarta]
    2. Jakarta
    1. -e
    2. -D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. (98)
    1.   murid-e
    2.   student-D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. [teko’
    2. from
    1. Jakarta]
    2. Jakarta
    1.    ‘Siti’s student from Jakarta’

And an adjective modifying the possessum also cannot occur in its usual postnominal position, as shown below in Madurese. The adjective cannot undergo head movement with the head N (100), nor is it possible for the adjective to be separated from the nominal it modifies (101).22 Like nominal complements, a periphrastic strategy must be used, in this case a relative structure (102):

    1. (99)
    1.    Madurese
    1.   mored
    2.   student
    1. tengghi
    2. tall
    1.    ‘a tall student’
    1. (100)
    1. *mored
    2.   student
    1. tengghi
    2. tall
    1. -nah
    2. -D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. (101)
    1. *mored-dhah
    2.   student-D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. tengghi
    2. tall
    1. (102)
    1.   mored-dhah
    2.   student-D
    1. Siti
    2. Siti
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. tengghi
    2. tall
    1.    ‘Siti’s student who is tall’

It is a stipulation that in these languages, adjectives and nominal complements cannot be stranded after head movement in possessive DPs. The fact that PPs may be stranded (94, 98) suggests that PPs may be adjoined higher in possessive DPs (when a PP and an adjective co-occur, the PP always occurs outside the adjective).

The patterns observed above apply across all three languages, and support the head movement shown in (88). This structure derives the surface word order for possessive DPs with nominal complements, PP adjuncts and adjectival modifiers.

In the first step of possessor extraction, the possessor in (103) undergoes movement driven by an edge feature [D] on the functional head D. This feature attracts the closest DP in its c-command domain, and raises it to its specifier to check [D].23 As previously mentioned, A-bar extraction is limited to nominals in these languages: only a DP may be clefted or relativized, whereas PP, AP and other adjuncts cannot.

    1. (103)
    1. Possessor sub-extraction from DP

All extracted nominals are focused, so the feature [D] probes together with a [Focus] feature. I have previously shown that either the matrix DP or its possessor may be extracted. It is the feature [Focus] that determines whether the entire possessive DP or its possessor is extracted. The interpretive difference between these has to do with focus:

    1. (104)
    1. Madurese
    1. Padha-nah
    2. foot-D
    1. pak
    2. Mr
    1. Djoko
    2. Djoko
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. e-obhadh-ih
    2. PASS-medicine-APPL
    1. sareng
    2. by
    1. dokter.
    2. doctor
    1. ‘It was Mr Djoko’s foot that was treated by the doctor.’
    1. (105)
    1. Pak
    2. Mr
    1. Djoko
    2. Djoko
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. padha-nah
    2. foot-D
    1. e-obhadh-ih
    2. PASS-medicine-APPL
    1. sareng
    2. by
    1. dokter.
    2. doctor
    1. ‘It was Mr Djoko that (his) foot was treated by the doctor.’

In (104), the focused DP is ‘Mr Djoko’s foot’ whereas in (105) the focus is solely on the possessor ‘Mr Djoko.’ In the first case, the matrix DP bears [Focus]; in the second, the possessor DP that has raised to the specifier bears [Focus]. Since [D] and [Focus] probe together, the closest goal must also bear both [D] and [Focus].

Movement to the edge of the DP is required for a possessor to be extracted. It is worth mentioning that the possessor does not surface this position, at the left edge of the DP. The reason is that movement obtains through null Operator movement. Recall that extraction in these languages employs a cleft strategy in which a null Operator undergoes movement inside a relative clause; the clefted nominal is generated separately as the predicate of a copular clause (see Section 2.3). Thus, in (103) it is a null Operator that raises to SpecDP before moving on to the next phase edge, SpecVoiceP. (The possessor cannot be pronounced at the edge of the VoiceP phase either.) The clefted DP can only be pronounced sentence initially or sentence finally because it is generated external to the relative clause.

When the possessor moves through SpecDP, it triggers obligatory morphology on the possessum, the suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah. I take these suffixes to be a type of wh-agreement that occurs in possessor extraction. Cross-linguistically, wh-agreement appears as special morphology that is a reflex of A-bar movement (or wh-movement). Wh-agreement morphology frequently occurs as a morpheme that is used elsewhere in the language, but without semantic content; it merely reflects syntactic movement. This is consistent with -nya/-ne/-Nah: as a result of possessor A-bar movement through the edge of DP, the possessum must occur with the suffix, which marks movement but does not carry definite semantics. Although wh-agreement has been frequently observed in complementizer systems, it has also been attested in the verbal domain (Zaenen 1983; Georgopoulos 1985; Tuller 1986; Chung & Georgopoulos 1988; Haik 1990; Watanabe 1996; Chung 1998; Reintges et al. 2006). I propose that possessor extraction in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese exhibits wh-agreement within the DP.

Wh-agreement differs from other types of morphological agreement in that it typically does not register phi features of the moved argument. If -nya is a type of wh-agreement, -nya is predicted to be compatible with 1 and 2 person possessor extraction. As the examples below show, these suffixes can indeed occur with 1/2 possessors:

    1. (106)
    1.    Indonesian
    1. %Aku/kamu
    2.   1SG/2SG
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. rumah-nya
    2. house-D
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1.    ‘It is I/you whose house was destroyed yesterday.’
    1. (107)
    1. %Rumah-ku/rumah-mu
    2.   house-1SG/house-2SG
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1.    ‘It is my house/your house that was destroyed yesterday.’
    1. (108)
    1.    Javanese
    1. %Aku/kamu
    2.   1SG/2SG
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-ne
    2. book-D
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru.
    2. blue
    1.    ‘It is I/you whose book is blue.’
    1. (109)
    1.   Buku-ku/buku-mu
    2.   book-1SG/book-2SG
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru.
    2. blue
    1.    ‘It is my book/your book that is blue.’
    1. (110)
    1.    Madurese (familiar)
    1. *Engko’/
    2.   1SG
    1. %hedah
    2.   2SG
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’.
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    ‘It is I/you whose book was read by little brother.’
    1. (111)
    1.   Tang
    2.   1SG.GEN
    1. buku/
    2. book
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. hedah
    2. 2SG
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’.
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    ‘It is my book/your book that was read by little brother.’

As indicated by the symbol %, some speakers find 1 or 2 possessor extraction very unusual in Indonesian and Javanese, and prefer pied-piping of the possessum as in (107) and (109).24 In Madurese, 1 possessors cannot be extracted, and there is disagreement about extraction of 2 possessors.25 However, all speakers agree that given an appropriate information-structural context for clefting of a 1/2 possessor, the suffix -nya/-ne/-Nah is obligatory.26

This is consistent with an analysis of -nya/-ne/-Nah as wh-agreement, which does not register the phi features of a moved argument. If the suffix reflected phi features, we might expect that 1/2 clitic pronouns could occur instead of -nya/-ne/-Nah. However, this is not the case, as shown below in Indonesian and Javanese:

    1. (112)
    1.    Indonesian
    1. *Aku
    2.   1SG
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. rumah-ku
    2. house-1SG
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1.    (‘It is I whose house was destroyed yesterday.’)
    1. (113)
    1. *Kamu
    2.   2SG
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. rumah-mu
    2. house-2SG
    1. di-rata-kan
    2. PASS-flat-APPL
    1. kemarin.
    2. yesterday
    1.    (‘It is you whose house was destroyed yesterday.’)
    1. (114)
    1.    Javanese
    1. *Aku
    2.   1SG
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-ku
    2. book-1SG
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru.
    2. blue
    1.    (‘It is I whose book is blue.’)
    1. (115)
    1. *Kamu
    2.   2SG
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. buku-mu
    2. book-2SG
    1. werno
    2. color
    1. biru.
    2. blue
    1.    (‘It is you whose book is blue.’)

Madurese does not have separate clitic forms for pronouns, but a (resumptive) 1 or 2 pronoun is not possible instead of -Nah:

    1. (116)
    1.    Madurese (familiar)
    1. *Engko’
    2.   1SG
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. tang
    2. 1SG.GEN
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’.
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    (‘It is I whose book was read by little brother.’)
    1. (117)
    1. *Hedah
    2.   2SG
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. buku
    2. book
    1. hedah
    2. 2SG
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’.
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    (‘It is you whose book was read by little brother.’)

I have proposed that extraction of a possessor proceeds through the edge of the DP, and that this movement triggers wh-agreement within DP. In the following section I extend this idea to VoiceP: in all three languages, extraction through the edge of VoiceP is marked by wh-agreement on the head Voice in active transitive clauses.

4.2 Possessors extract through SpecVoiceP

A possessor that has first moved to the specifier of a possessive DP, next undergoes successive-cyclic movement through the edge of VoiceP, before landing in its surface position in the CP domain. I assume that the extended verbal structure of the clause includes both vP and VoiceP, which I take to be the highest verbal projection and the domain relevant for successive-cyclic movement, or a phase.27 The voice prefixes on the verb are the spellout of the functional head Voice. In basic active clauses, this head is realized as meN- in Indonesian, N- in Javanese, N- or a- in Madurese.28 It is this head that is also phonologically null when there is extraction from object position. External arguments are generated in the specifier of VoiceP (Pylkkänen 2008; Harley 2013; Legate 2014), and raised to the position of grammatical subjects, SpecIP, to satisfy [EPP] on I.2930

In an active clause, the derivation of possessor extraction from object position begins with an active Voice head that bears the features [D] and [Focus]. The [D] feature was previously discussed for DP-internal movement: it attracts the closest DP to its specifier, but does not target other categories such as PP or AP. [D] on Voice triggers phase-based movement through the edge of the verbal domain. This type of movement is proposed in Chomsky (1986; 2000; through the edge of vP) and also implemented in various analyses of Indonesian and Austronesian languages (Rackowski & Richards 2005; Aldridge 2008; Cole et al. 2008; Sato 2008; 2012; Legate 2014; van Urk & Richards 2015; Aldridge 2017). These analyses share the view that one of the functions of voice morphology is to mark nominal movement through the edge of the phase.

For nominals in object position, [D] and [Focus] probe together to attract the closest DP goal that bears both [D] and [Focus]. This DP is raised to SpecVoiceP. This is illustrated in (119) with the Indonesian possessor orang ‘person.’

    1. (118)
    1. Indonesian
    1. orang
    2. person
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. adik
    2. younger.sibling
    1. baca
    2. read
    1. buku-nya
    2. book-D
    1. ‘the person that little brother read (his) book’
    1. (119)
    1. Possessor extraction from object position in active clause313233

Once the possessor is raised to the edge of the phase in SpecVoiceP, it is available for further movement. The possessor must first move to the edge of DP, then to the edge of VoiceP, in order to be visible to a probe on C; nominals within a lower phase are not visible for movement operations. Although Voice in active clauses is usually pronounced as the prefix meN-/N-/a-, when Voice bears the feature [D], triggering obligatory movement, it is realized as a null prefix (for related discussion of the null prefix in cases of extraction, see Cole & Hermon 2005; Cole et al. 2008; Sato 2008; 2012).

This analysis holds for Indonesian, Javanese and polite Madurese, which allow object extraction and object possessor extraction. In familiar Madurese however, since objects and their possessors cannot extract, the active Voice head does not bear the [D] feature. Nominals cannot raise to the edge of VoiceP from object position, and consequently are not visible to a probe on C, which cannot probe into a lower phase. As a result, objects and their possessors cannot be extracted in familiar Madurese; and since Voice never bears the [D] feature, active verbs always bear a voice prefix.

For the external argument (and its possessor) in an active clause, movement to a higher position does not require [D] on the Voice head, because the external argument is generated in the specifier of Voice and therefore is already on the edge of the phase; it is visible for further movement. The [EPP] feature on I first raises the external argument to the grammatical subject position in SpecIP, where it may then be found by a probe on C. Since the external argument in an active clause does not interact with [D] on the Voice head, extraction of this nominal never has consequences for voice morphology; a bare verb is not required for subject extraction. Similarly, the internal argument (and its possessor) in a passive clause are first raised to grammatical subject, and can be extracted from SpecIP. The passive voice morphology is required when the internal argument occurs as subject; however, further extraction from subject position is not reflected by a change of verbal morphology.

5 Implications of possessor extraction

5.1 Phase heads D and Voice

Since movement through vP and CP was proposed in Chomsky (2000; 2001), successive-cyclic movement based on phases has been implemented in analyses of many other languages (for discussion in Indonesian-type languages, see Aldridge 2008; Cole et al. 2008; Sato 2008; 2012; Legate 2014; Aldridge 2017). I have provided novel data from possessor extraction that also supports phase-cyclic movement: in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese, possessor extraction requires local movement through phases. The possessor undergoes A-bar movement through SpecDP, then SpecVoiceP, before landing in CP. The evidence for this type of successive-cyclic movement includes the suffix required on the possessum (in Indonesian), which marks DP-internal A-bar movement, and the null prefix on the verb (in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese), which marks A-bar movement through VoiceP. The implication is that D and Voice are phase heads in Indonesian-type languages; the edge feature [D] on a phase head triggers obligatory movement of a nominal through its edge.

In Section 4.1 I argued that Indonesian -nya is a type of morphological wh-agreement in cases of possessor extraction. Extending this to the verbal domain, the (lack of) active verbal morphology in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese also has the properties of wh-agreement: A-bar movement over the verb triggers a special form (i.e. a null prefix). The null prefix has no semantic content (i.e. the verb remains active and transitive), but merely marks syntactic movement. One implication of this view is that morphological wh-agreement correlates with phases in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese; wh-agreement surfaces as realization (or null realization) of the phase heads Voice and D. To my knowledge, wh-agreement in the DP domain has not been reported in other languages (see Watanabe 1996; Reintges et al. 2006 for cross-linguistic generalizations about wh-agreement). The Indonesian data present here provide novel evidence for the phasehood of D, as well as morphological wh-agreement within the DP.

5.2 Revisiting A-bar extraction in Indonesian-type languages

Possessor extraction also brings new insight to analyses of Indonesian-type voice systems. First, the head Voice does not only determine the argument structure of the clause (i.e. transitivity; or whether an external argument is hosted in its specifier), but also the movement of DPs. Since the possessor is not an argument of the verb, but rather a possessive argument of N, it is interesting that movement of the possessor is constrained by Voice in the same way that verbal arguments are. If the voice system regulates movement of possessor DPs, then Voice is concerned with all DPs that shift out of VoiceP, not just verbal arguments. Whereas morphological voice marking typically indicates the position of verbal arguments (for example, the Theme in an active clause remains low, while the Theme in a passive clause occurs as subject), when nominal extraction has occurred in an active clause, morphological voice marking also serves as a reflex of nominal movement through VoiceP. When sub-extraction of the possessor from object position occurs in Indonesian, Javanese and polite Madurese, the null voice prefix is required (just as when the full object DP is extracted). The object of the verb has not shifted, since the head N, the possessum remains in its merged position. Yet extraction of a non-argument, the possessor, requires that the voice morphology reflect that a DP has moved through the edge of VoiceP.

This discussion about possessor extraction calls for a re-examination of analyses of DP extraction. For example, Cole, Hermon & Yanti (2008) propose that in Indonesian, either the case or the thematic role of the shifted nominal agrees with the morphological voice marker on the verb; a morphological filter prevents conflicting features on the verb and the extracted DP. Possessors present a challenge to this type of analysis because both the object (matrix DP) and its possessor can be extracted with a null voice marker. Assuming that abstract genitive or possessive case is assigned to the possessor, while its matrix DP bears abstract accusative case, both types of nominals could not extract with the same null voice prefix. A similar argument applies to a mismatch in theta roles between an argument and its possessor. This analysis faces further challenges when a subject undergoes long distance extraction from an embedded clause (see Saddy 1991 for extended discussion). Long-distance subject movement from an embedded clause requires a bare verb in the matrix clause, collapsing the case/thematic distinction between subjects and objects.

Despite the difficulties that possessor extraction poses to this particular analysis, the general proposal set forth by Cole, Hermon & Yanti is that voice morphology in Indonesian instantiates a type of agreement. I have also suggested that voice morphology is a type of agreement in Indonesian, i.e. wh-agreement, and that this analysis applies to Javanese and Madurese as well. Since wh-agreement does not reflect phi features cross-linguistically, and often does not register case or thematic features, the mismatches mentioned above do not pose a problem. When nominals undergo A-bar movement through the edge of VoiceP in active clauses, the null voice prefix is a reflex of this movement.

5.3 Implications for the left periphery in Indonesian-type languages

Possessor extraction also provides new observations about the organization of the left periphery of the clause. I have argued that the availability of possessor extraction from object position, and the extraction of objects in general, is regulated low in the clause, by Voice. If a nominal can escape VoiceP (or is already on the edge of VoiceP), then it can undergo A-bar extraction. Languages like familiar Madurese, in which subjects can be extracted but objects cannot, do not allow Voice to bear an edge feature [D] that raises nominals to SpecVoiceP.

Other theories offer an alternative explanation for the impossibility of object extraction in a language such as familiar Madurese. Some authors have accounted for “subject-only” extraction in some Austronesian languages (like familiar Madurese) by invoking some variation of feature inheritance (Chomsky 2005; 2008; Fortuny 2008; Legate 2011; 2014). Inheritance is motivated by the close relationship between C and T cross-linguistically. In Austronesian languages, feature inheritance is further motivated by the properties of the highest or leftmost argument, which appears to have both A and A-bar properties, which might follow if C and T are not distinct in these languages (e.g. Pearson 2005; Rackowski & Richards 2005).

Inheritance accounts for the impossibility of object extraction in familiar Madurese in the following way. Legate (2011; 2014) proposes under-inheritance, in which the formal features on C can fail to be inherited by T. The result is that CP and TP are not projected separately, but rather form a single combined projection: let us call this CTP. When the specifier of CTP is filled (by a subject or topic), no structural position is available for another DP to raise to. Therefore, object extraction is impossible in active clauses, because the external argument already occupies the single position available at the left periphery. The effect of under-inheritance is that only one nominal can occur in a high position in the clause in familiar Madurese and other Austronesian languages with “subject-only” extraction.

Alternatively, recent theories of head-splitting (Martinović 2015; see also Erlewine 2017) posit that some of the features on a functional head can split off and re-project a new head. Head-splitting of CTP results in the traditional division between TP and CP. If this head does not split however, the single head (CT) could host only one argument in its specifier; just as with under-inheritance, a preverbal subject would prevent another argument from moving to a high position.

If under-inheritance or failure of head-splitting is correct for a language that does not allow any object extraction, then we expect that familiar Madurese could not host two arguments at the left periphery of the clause, i.e. one argument in CP and another in TP(IP). Recall that objects cannot extract in familiar Madurese; (58) is repeated here as (120):

    1. (120)
    1.    Madurese (familiar)
    1. *Sapah
    2.   who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. ale’
    2. younger.sibling
    1. m-acah/bacah
    2. ACTV-read/read
    1. buku-nah?
    2. book-D
    1.    (‘Who was it that little brother read his book?’)

Under a theory of under-inheritance or failure of head-splitting, (120) is not possible because the subject position is filled, so no structural position exists as a landing place for an extracted possessor. However, this also predicts that possessor extraction from subject position is impossible, since an argument already fills the specifier of CTP. We have already seen that this prediction is not borne out: two distinct positions do exist at the left periphery in familiar Madurese; (3) is repeated here as (121):

    1. (121)
    1. Madurese (familiar)
    1. Sapah
    2. who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. buku-nah
    2. book-D
    1. e-bacah
    2. PASS-read
    1. ale’?
    2. younger.sibling
    1. ‘Whose book was read by little brother?’

The possessor and the possessum simultaneously occur in separate A and A-bar positions, the latter separated from the rest of the clause by the relative morpheme se. Possessor extraction, therefore, shows that under-inheritance and head splitting do not account for the impossibility of object extraction, at least in familiar Madurese. In familiar Madurese, a traditional split between C and T(I) remains, with (at least) two positions for nominals at the left periphery of the clause (SpecCP and SpecIP). For other languages of the area that do not allow object extraction, possessor extraction may provide a useful diagnostic to test whether the clause has two structural positions high at the left periphery.

6 Conclusion

This paper investigates novel possessor extraction patterns in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese that have not previously been reported in the literature. I have shown that syntactic movement, rather than base generation, derives the surface position of external possessors in these languages. I have argued that the suffix on the possessum is not a resumptive pronoun, but rather pronunciation of the head D. Under this novel view, there is no 3 possessive clitic in these languages; rather, a pro possessor with -nya/-ne/-Nah derives a 3 person interpretation. Possessors first escape their possessive DPs by A-bar movement through the specifier of DP, then the specifier of VoiceP. The null voice prefix on an active verb not only marks the extraction of an object, but must also mark the extraction of a possessor from object position. This is evidence that the functional head Voice regulates A-bar extraction of all nominals passing through its specifier.

Possessor extraction provides support for D and Voice as phase heads in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese. I have proposed that phase-cyclic syntactic movement is driven by the features [D] and [Focus] on phase heads Voice and D. Furthermore, I have argued that obligatory pronunciation of the nominal suffixes -nya/-ne/-Nah and the obligatory null voice prefix in all three languages are a type of morphological wh-agreement triggered by A-bar movement. Another implication of possessor extraction data is that the organization of the left periphery in these languages shows a structural distinction between CP and TP(IP), with a traditional division of features associated with C and T. In sum, this paper has attempted to bring novel possessor extraction data to the rich discussion on voice and nominal extraction in Indonesian-type languages.

6.1 Remaining issues

This paper has focused on sub-extraction from subject and object positions. The analysis predicts that possessor sub-extraction will possible only from positions that allow DP extraction. Here I briefly explore this possibility in complex NPs and adjunct clauses in Indonesian, as well as ditransitives in Indonesian and Javanese.34

In previous examples (13–16), we have already seen that DPs may not extract out of complex NPs or adjunct clauses in Indonesian, which predicts that sub-extraction of possessors should not be possible from the same environments. The prediction is borne out:

    1. (122)
    1.    Indonesian
    1.   Susan
    2.   Susan
    1. dapat
    2. get
    1. kesimpulan
    2. conclusion
    1. bahwa
    2. that
    1. Lani
    2. Lani
    1. lebih
    2. more
    1. suka
    2. like
    1. kue
    2. cake
    1. Fetty.
    2. Fetty
    1.    ‘Susan got the conclusion that Lani prefers Fetty’s cake.’
    1. (123)
    1. *Siapa
    2.   who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. Susan
    2. Susan
    1. dapat
    2. get
    1. kesimpulan
    2. conclusion
    1. Lani
    2. Lani
    1. lebih
    2. more
    1. suka
    2. like
    1. kue-nya?
    2. cake-D
    1.    (‘Who is it that Susan got the conclusion that Lani prefers their cake?’)
    1. (124)
    1.   Ayah
    2.   Father
    1. senang
    2. happy
    1. ketika
    2. when
    1. me-lihat
    2. ACTV-see
    1. piala
    2. trophy
    1. adik.
    2. younger.sibling
    1.    ‘Father was happy when he saw little brother’s trophy.’
    1. (125)
    1. *Siapa
    2.   who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. Ayah
    2. Father
    1. senang
    2. happy
    1. ketika
    2. when
    1. lihat
    2. see
    1. piala-nya?
    2. trophy-D
    1.    (‘Who is it that Father was happy when he saw their trophy?’)

Similar possessor sub-extraction from complex NPs and adjunct clauses is ruled out in Javanese and Madurese as well; but since all long-distance movement is not possible in these languages, the reason for the unacceptability of the possessor movement is not clear.

Next, I look at extraction from ditransitive clauses, beginning with Madurese. (127) and (128) show that Madurese does not allow extraction of a Recipient/Goal/Beneficiary, which I will call the applicative object. As expected, possessors may also not extract from this position (129).

    1. (126)
    1.    Madurese (polite)35 (Davies 2010: 283; glosses modified)
    1.   Embhuk
    2.   elder.sister
    1. ng-errem-eh
    2. ACTV-send-APPL
    1. ebuh
    2. mother
    1. paket.
    2. package
    1.    ‘Big sister sent Mother a package.’
    1. (127)
    1. *Sapah
    2.   who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. embhuk
    2. elder.sister
    1. kerrem-eh
    2. send-APPL
    1. paket?
    2. package
    1.    (‘Who did big sister send a package?)
    1. (128)
    1. *oreng
    2.   person
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. embhuk
    2. elder.sister
    1. kerrem-eh
    2. ACTV-send-APPL
    1. paket
    2. package
    1.    (‘the person that big sister sent a package’)
    1. (129)
    1. *Sapah
    2.   who
    1. se
    2. REL
    1. embhuk
    2. elder.sister
    1. kerrem-eh
    2. send-APPL
    1. ghuruh-nah
    2. teacher-D
    1. paket?
    2. package
    1.    (‘Who is it that big sister sent their teacher a package?)

Contrary to expectation, the correlation between argument extraction and possessor sub-extraction does not appear to hold for ditransitives. Indonesian and Javanese do allow extraction of an applicative object:

    1. (130)
    1. Indonesian (examples (130–133) modified from Sato 2012: 43)
    1. Kamu
    2. 2SG
    1. mem-beli-kan
    2. ACTV-buy-APPL
    1. ibu-mu
    2. mother-2SG
    1. bunga.
    2. flower
    1. ‘You bought your mother flowers.’
    1. (131)
    1. Siapa
    2. who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. kamu
    2. 2SG
    1. beli-kan/
    2. buy-APPL/
    1. *mem-beli-kan
    2.   ACTV-buy-APPL
    1. bunga?
    2. flower
    1. ‘Who did you buy flowers (for)?’
    1. (132)
    1. Javanese
    1. Kowe
    2. 2SG
    1. n-uko-kke
    2. ACTV-buy-APPL
    1. ibu-mu
    2. mother-2SG
    1. kembang.
    2. flower
    1. ‘You bought your mother flowers.’
    1. (133)
    1. Sopo
    2. who
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. kowe
    2. 2SG
    1. tuko-kke/
    2. buy-APPL/
    1. *n-uko-kke
    2.   ACTV-buy-APPL
    1. kembang?
    2. flower
    1. ‘Who did you buy flowers (for)?’

Note that extraction is only possible for a Beneficiary/Goal/Recipient, i.e. the higher of the two ditransitive arguments; extraction of the Theme ‘flower’ is not possible.36 Contrary to expectation, a possessor cannot be extracted from an applicative object that remains in situ:

    1. (134)
    1.    Indonesian
    1. *orang
    2.   person
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. kamu
    2. 2SG
    1. beli-kan
    2. buy-APPL
    1. istri-nya
    2. wife-D
    1. bunga
    2. flower
    1.    (‘the person that you bought his wife flowers’)
    1. (135)
    1. *Siapa
    2.   who
    1. yang
    2. REL
    1. kamu
    2. 2SG
    1. beli-kan
    2. buy-APPL
    1. istri-nya
    2. wife-D
    1. bunga?
    2. flower
    1.    (‘Who is it that you bought his wife flowers?’)
    1. (136)
    1.    Javanese
    1. *wong
    2.   person
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. kowe
    2. 2SG
    1. tuko-kke
    2. buy-APPL
    1. bojo-ne
    2. wife-D
    1. kembang
    2. flower
    1.    (‘the person that you bought his wife flowers’)
    1. (137)
    1. *Sopo
    2.   who
    1. sing
    2. REL
    1. kowe
    2. 2SG
    1. tuko-kke
    2. buy-APPL
    1. bojo-ne
    2. wife-D
    1. kembang?
    2. flower
    1.    (‘Who is it that you bought his wife flowers?’)

Sub-extraction of the possessor from the applicative object is not possible even though the verb does not bear the active voice prefix in (134–137). Thus the previously noted correlation, between the extractability of a subject or monotransitive object and the extractability of its possessor, does not extend to applicative objects in ditransitive clauses.

The impossibility of possessor extraction from the applicative object position, then, presents a puzzle: why cannot the possessor of the applicative object be extracted? At the present time this remains unanswered. However, I note that another thematic argument position shares this unexplained property: the base position of external arguments, or SpecVoiceP (cf. Pylkkänen 2008; Harley 2013; Legate 2014). In active clauses, a DP that is generated as an external argument in SpecVoiceP moves out of its thematic position and raises to subject position, SpecIP. In contrast, these languages have another clause type, an Object voice clause, in which the external argument remains in SpecVoiceP while the Theme becomes grammatical subject.37 In Object voice, the external argument cannot be moved at all, but remains in its thematic position. Possessor sub-extraction cannot be tested in Object voice in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese: the external argument in Object voice is restricted to a limited set of DPs, usually pronouns and names (see Chung 1976; Sneddon 1996; Nomoto 2006; Sneddon 2006; Cole et al. 2008; Jeoung 2017). This means that complex arguments such as possessive DPs are ruled out in this position. However, Musgrave (2001) shows that this external argument cannot launch floating quantifiers in Indonesian Object voice. In related languages, Legate (2014) demonstrates the impossibility of floating quantifiers from an Agent in Acehnese Object voice, and Arka (2003) presents similar observations in Balinese. This thematic position, then, does not appear to allow sub-extraction in a variety of languages. Like the base position of applicative objects, it is not well understood why SpecVoiceP does not allow this type of movement. I leave this as an open issue that requires further investigation.

Abbreviations

APPL = applicative, ACTV = active voice, CAUS = causative, D = definite morpheme, INCL = inclusive, INTR = intransitive, INTENS = intensifier, INVOL = involitive, NEG = negation, NMLZ = nominalizer, PASS = passive voice, PERF = perfective, REL = relative

Notes

  1. Data are from my fieldwork notes unless another source is cited. The Indonesian and Javanese data given in this paper are from the colloquial varieties spoken in East Java. My Madurese consultants are from Bangkalan and Jember; however, all Madurese data cited in this paper are from the Bangkalan (western) variety. [^]
  2. Chung (2008) cites examples of a possessor occurring at the left edge of the clause without a relative morpheme, such as the copular clause in (i). However, the prosody suggests that the possessor in this case is a left-dislocated topic.
      1. (i)
      1. Cited from Wolff et al. (1992: 125)
      1. Orang
      2. person
      1. itu,
      2. that
      1. delapan
      2. eight
      1. anak-nya.
      2. child-D
      1. ‘That person has eight children.’
    [^]
  3. Pseudo-clefts occur without an overt expletive element and copula, yet have a clefted interpretation. [^]
  4. For Indonesian (and Malay), the distribution of focus particles also supports the predicate status of the clefted nominal. The focus particle -lah and the interrogative particle -kah may attach to predicates, but not subjects (Cole, Hermon & Aman 1999; Musgrave 2001). In (26–27) these particles may attach to Siti, but not to the headless relative. [^]
  5. External wh possessors must be animate; this restriction does not apply to external lexical possessors. [^]
  6. Sneddon 2006 reports that this rule is relaxed in colloquial Jakartan Indonesian. [^]
  7. For my Indonesian consultants, in-situ subject questions are possible with siapa ‘who,’ if the questioned individual is included among the addressees: ‘Who (among you)…’ The necessary context for this suggests that D-linking may play a role in subject wh-in situ (cf. Pesetsky 1987). [^]
  8. In basic Indonesian active clauses without object extraction, pronunciation of the ACTV prefix on many verbs appears optional (though this is debated). This variability is not addressed here. Where the ACTV prefix is shown in the data, it indicates that its presence is grammatical; this crucially contrasts with clauses that have object extraction, where pronunciation of an ACTV prefix results in ungrammaticality. [^]
  9. While maos ‘read’ is used only in the formal register, bacah ‘read’ can be used in both familiar and polite registers. [^]
  10. It is possible that the various registers in Javanese have differences in extraction, but this has not yet been reported in the literature. Registers in Indonesian are less clearly differentiated than in Madurese and Javanese, but to my knowledge, extraction does not differ according to politeness. [^]
  11. There is competition between the use of a free pronoun and clitic in Indonesian possessive DPs. In colloquial speech, speakers prefer the clitic, but also find the free pronoun possible. [^]
  12. The co-occurrence of -nya with a pronominal possessor is reported to be more common in emphatic or affective contexts; see also footnote 19 for Javanese. [^]
  13. The Javanese suffix is listed as -ne, but as already mentioned, it can variably occur as –e in the same positions. [^]
  14. Like Indonesian, the co-occurrence of -ne with a pronominal possessor is not common in Javanese, but is associated with emphatic or affective contexts. [^]
  15. Javanese and Madurese have considerable regional variation in personal pronouns, as well as dedicated sets of pronouns for different registers (see e.g. Uhlenbeck 1978; Suharno 1982; Errington 1998 for Javanese; Davies 2010 for Madurese). Table 1 is not intended to be a comprehensive summary of the pronominal system in each language; this subset of singular pronouns is used to illustrate the main points under discussion. [^]
  16. Clitic possessors may compete with the suffix for a single position; it is also possible that after -nya/-ne/-Nah a strong form of the pronoun is required for prosodic reasons. [^]
  17. My consultant reports that using the 1 person free pronoun as a possessor, i.e. roma-nah engko’, is possible in some dialects of Madurese. [^]
  18. Note that for non-possessive instances of -nya, I have used a neutral gloss of -NYA instead of -D. [^]
  19. In (81–82), -nya appears to replace a 3 person argument. For possessives, I have proposed that a pro possessor requires pronunciation of -nya. This idea might be extended more generally, such that -nya marks the presence of a pro argument in (81–82). I leave this as an avenue for future research. [^]
  20. The deictic pronoun itu is used here instead of 3SG dia because dia is reserved for human (or animate) arguments. [^]
  21. Only Indonesian is used to illustrate this point because Indonesian allows the suffix -nya to cliticize to active verbs; this is not possible in Javanese and Madurese. [^]
  22. My consultants have judgments that deviate from the Madurese data reported in Davies & Dresser 2005, where adjectives may occur between the possessum and possessor; my consultants find these examples ill-formed. The few exceptions are idiomatic compounds such as oreng tua ‘parents’ (literal: ‘old person’), which occurs inside the suffix in oreng tua-nah Siti ‘Siti’s parents’ (literal: ‘Siti’s old people’). [^]
  23. Movement of Pak Djoko is shown for expository purposes here, although I assume that this movement obtains via null Operator movement (Section 2.3); see further discussion below. [^]
  24. Disagreements among consultants about the acceptability of 1 or 2 possessor extraction may be caused by the overall infrequency of possessor sub-extraction, and its typical occurrence with 3 possessors. I do not know of other languages in which 3 possessors may sub-extract while 1 and 2 possessors may not. [^]
  25. It is not surprising that 1 possessors cannot sub-extract at all in Madurese, as shown in (110). Recall that the 1 singular possessive pronoun in Madurese is unusual: tang is a proclitic form used only in genitive constructions, for a 1 singular possessor. This is illustrated in (111), where the possessive DP is tang buku ‘my book’ rather than buku-nah engko’. This is likely the reason that extraction of engko’ is ruled out. [^]
  26. The declarative pseudo-cleft construction requires the information in the relative clause to be presupposed. For example, the required context for (106) is as follows: Speaker A returns from a trip and remarks that he heard that a house in his neighborhood had been destroyed. He wonders if the owner of the house knows what happened. Speaker B replies, Aku/kamu yang rumahnya diratakan kemarin ‘It is I/you whose house was destroyed yesterday!’ [^]
  27. Causative prefixes in these languages are hosted in vP (cf. Legate 2014). These prefixes do not interact with possessor extraction. [^]
  28. See Davies 2010 on the distribution of N- and a- in Madurese, which is not relevant for the present discussion. [^]
  29. In active voice, the derived subject position generally must be filled in these languages. VP fronting is also possible in active voice, resulting in variations in word order, but does not occur with the clefts and relatives that are discussed here. [^]
  30. I use IP instead of TP because tense is not overtly marked in Indonesian clauses. [^]
  31. Note that in (119), DP-internal movement of the possessor is not shown; see discussion surrounding (103). The possessor orang is shown for expository purposes; possessor movement obtains via a null Operator, as previously discussed in Section 2.3. [^]
  32. In (119) the possessor is tucked in (cf. Richards 1999) below the specifier hosting the external argument. Assuming [EPP] on I targets the closest DP eligible for movement, the external argument rather than the possessor is raised to SpecIP. [^]
  33. (119) shows the root baca ‘read’ in the node V. The formation of the phonological word that includes the heads Voice-v-V is not shown here. [^]
  34. Thanks to an anonymous reviewer for comments that led to the development of this discussion. [^]
  35. These sentences can be used in either familiar or polite speech, as the verb kerrem is compatible with both. Neither register allows extraction of the Recipient/Goal/Beneficiary. [^]
  36. The judgments of my consultants differ from those reported in Sato 2012. For Sato’s consultants, the Theme can be extracted over the Goal/Recipient in formal/Standard Indonesian and Kendal Javanese: see his examples (18b, 19b). This is not possible for the Indonesian and Javanese speakers that I consulted; similar extraction is also impossible with “give”-type ditransitives (cf. Kaswanti Purwo 1995); examples are omitted due to space considerations. [^]
  37. The properties of Indonesian Object voice are described in Chung (1976); Guilfoyle et al. (1992); Cole et al. (2008). In Javanese, only certain varieties may have Object voice (see Cole, Jonczyk & Lilley 1999; Sato 2012). In Madurese, Object voice is used only in the Polite register (see Jeoung 2017). [^]

Acknowledgements

Thanks to my consultants (alphabetized by first given name): Gabriella Melody, Hainona Izza Golia, Iline Megale, Ilkham Hidayat, Isya Mahfud, M. Khoirun Najib, Maimuna, Nadhilah H. Semendawai; and grateful appreciation to Lilis Lestari Wilujeng for logistical support in fieldwork. Thanks to David Embick, Julie Legate and Florian Schwarz for helpful comments and suggestions on various stages of this work. Part of this work was funded by a GAPSA-Provost Award for Interdisciplinary Innovation and a Teece Dissertation Research Fellowship from the University of Pennsylvania.

Competing Interests

The author has no competing interests to declare.

References

Aldridge, Edith. 2008. Phase-based account of extraction in Indonesian. Lingua 118(10). 1440–1469. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2007.08.006

Aldridge, Edith. 2014. Predicate, subject, and cleft in Austronesian languages. Sophia Linguistica 61. 97–121.

Aldridge, Edith. 2017. ϕ-feature competition: A unified approach to the Austronesian extraction restriction. In Proceedings of the 52nd meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS) 52.

Arka, I. Wayan. 2002. Voice systems in the Austronesian languages of Nusantara: Typology, symmetricality and undergoer orientation. http://hdl.handle.net/1885/41058.

Arka, I. Wayan. 2003. Balinese morphosyntax: A lexical-functional approach (Pacific Linguistics 547). Canberra: The Australian National University.

Arka, I. Wayan. 2013. On the typology and syntax of TAM in Indonesian. In John Bowden (ed.), Tense, aspect, mood and evidentiality in languages of Indonesia (NUSA 55), 23–40. Jakarta: Atma Jaya Catholic University.

Arka, I. Wayan & Christopher Manning. 2008. Voice and grammatical relations in Indonesian: A new perspective. In Peter K. Austin & Simon Musgrave (eds.), Voice and grammatical relations in Austronesian languages. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

Baker, Mark C. 1988. Incorporation: A theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Bell, Sarah J. 1983. Advancements and ascensions in Cebuano. In David M. Perlmutter (ed.), Studies in Relational Grammar 1. 143–218. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Blust, Robert. 2013. The Austronesian languages. Asia-Pacific Linguistics: Open Access Monographs. http://hdl.handle.net/1885/146289.

Chomsky, Noam. 1977. On wh movement. In Peter W. Culicover, Thomas Wasow & Adrian Akmajian (eds.), Formal syntax, 71–132. New York: Academic Press.

Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Barriers 13. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Roger Martin, David Michaels & Juan Uriagereka (eds.), Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, 89–155. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Michael Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, Noam. 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36. 1–22. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1162/0024389052993655

Chomsky, Noam. 2008. On phases. In Robert Freidin, Carlos P. Otero & Maria Luisa Zubizarreta (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory, 133–166. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262062787.003.0007

Chung, Sandra. 1976. On the subject of two passives in Indonesian. In Charles N. Li (ed.), Subject and topic, 57–98. New York: Academic Press.

Chung, Sandra. 1998. The design of agreement: Evidence from Chamorro. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Chung, Sandra. 2008. Possessors and definiteness effects in two Austronesian languages. In Lisa Matthewson (ed.), Quantification: A cross-linguistic perspective (North Holland Linguistic Series: Linguistic variations 64), 179–224. Chicago, IL: Emerald.

Chung, Sandra & Carol Georgopoulous. 1988. Agreement with gaps in Chamorro and Palauan. In Michael Barlow & Charles A. Ferguson (eds.), Agreement in natural language, 251–267. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

Cole, Peter, Elizabeth Jonczyk & Jason Lilley. 1999. A note on extraction from object position in Javanese and other Javanic languages. In Proceedings of AFLA 6, 87–93.

Cole, Peter & Gabriella Hermon. 1998. The typology of wh-movement: Wh-questions in Malay. Syntax 1(3). 221–258. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9612.00009

Cole, Peter & Gabriella Hermon. 2005. Subject and non-subject relativization in Indonesian. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 14(1). 59–88. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-004-2703-3

Cole, Peter, Gabriella Hermon, Kozue Inoha & Yassir Tjung. 2002. A constraint on wh in situ in Javanese. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 44. 91–106.

Cole, Peter, Gabriella Hermon & Norhaida Aman. 1999. Clefted questions in Malay. Ms.

Cole, Peter, Gabriella Hermon & Yanti. 2008. Voice in Malay/Indonesian. Lingua 118(10). 1500–1553. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2007.08.008

Coon, Jessica. 2009. Interrogative possessors and the problem with pied-piping in Chol. Linguistic Inquiry 40(1). 165–175. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1162/ling.2009.40.1.165

Davies, William D. 2000. Against long movement in Madurese. In Proceedings of AFLA 7, 33–48.

Davies, William D. 2003. Extreme locality in Madurese wh-questions. Syntax 6(3). 237–259. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9612.2003.00062.x

Davies, William D. 2010. A grammar of Madurese. New York: DeGruyter Mouton. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110224443

Davies, William D. & Craig A. Dresser. 2005. The structure of Javanese and Madurese determiner phrases. UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics 12. 57–72.

Deal, Amy Rose. 2013. External possession and possessor raising. In Martin Everaert & Henk C. van Riemsdijk (eds.), The Wiley Blackwell companion to syntax, 1–32. 2nd edition; Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

Erlewine, Michael Yoshitaka. 2017. Why the null complementizer is special in complementizer-trace effects. In Claire Halpert, Hadas Kotek & Coppe van Urk (eds.), A pesky set: Papers for David Pesetsky 80. 371–380. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Errington, James J. 1998. Shifting languages: Javanese Indonesian interaction and identity 19. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511612480

Fortuny, Jordi. 2008. The emergence of order in syntax 119. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/la.119

Gavruseva, Elena. 2000. On the syntax of possessor extraction. Lingua 110(10). 743–772. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(00)00015-2

Georgopoulos, Carol. 1985. Variables in Palauan syntax. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3(1). 59–94. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00205414

Guilfoyle, Eithne, Henrietta Hung & Lisa Travis. 1992. Spec of IP and Spec of VP: Two subjects in Austronesian languages. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 10(3). 375–414. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00133368

Haik, Isabelle. 1990. Anaphoric, pronominal and referential Infl. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 8(3). 347–374. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00135618

Harley, Heidi. 2013. External arguments and the Mirror Principle: On the distinctness of Voice and v. Lingua 125. 34–57. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2012.09.010

Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 2002. Voice in western Austronesian: An update. In The history and typology of western Austronesian voice systems, 7–16. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.

Jeoung, Helen. 2017. On the number of voices in Madurese. Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society 10(1). 16–35.

Kader, Mashudi B. H. 1976. The syntax of Malay interrogatives. Burnaby, BC: Simon Fraser University dissertation.

Kaswanti Purwo, Bambang. 1995. The two proto-types of ditransitive verbs: The Indonesian evidence. In Werner Abraham, Talmy Givón & Sandra A. Thompson (eds.), Discourse grammar and typology: Papers in honor of John W. M. Verhaar, 77–99. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Keenan, Edward L. 1972. Relative clause formation in Malagasy. The Chicago Which Hunt 169(89). 348–71.

Kroeger, Paul. 1993. Phrase structure and grammatical relations in Tagalog. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

Kroeger, Paul. 2009. Malagasy clefts from a western Malayo-Polynesian perspective: Comments on the paper by Hans-Martin Gartner. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 27(4). 817–838. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-009-9082-4

Kroeger, Paul. 2014. External negation in Malay/Indonesian. Language 90(1). 137–184. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2014.0000

Landau, Idan. 1999. Possessor raising and the structure of VP. Lingua 107. 1–37. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00025-4

Legate, Julie Anne. 2011. Under-inheritance. Paper presented at the 41st meeting of the North East Linguistic Society (NELS). University of Toronto.

Legate, Julie Anne. 2014. Voice and v: Lessons from Acehnese. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262028141.001.0001

Martinović, Martina. 2015. Feature geometry and head-splitting: Evidence from the morphosyntax of the Wolof clausal periphery. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago dissertation.

Massam, Diane. 2003. Questions and the left periphery in Niuean. Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics 19. 94–106.

McCloskey, James. 2001. The morphosyntax of wh-extraction in Irish. Journal of Linguistics 37(1). 67–100. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226701008775

McCloskey, James. 2002. Resumption, successive cyclicity and the locality of operations. In Samuel D. Epstein & T. Daniel Seeley (eds.), Derivation and Explanation in the Minimalist Program 5. 184–226. Malden, MA: Blackwell. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1002/9780470755662.ch9

Musgrave, Simon. 2001. Non-subject arguments in Indonesian. Melbourne: University of Melbourne dissertation.

Nomoto, Hiroki. 2006. Voice in colloquial Malay relatives. In Yoichiro Tsuruga, Nobuyuki Miyake & Yuji Kawaguchi (eds.), Gengojouhougaku Kenkyuuhoukoku 12. 97–116. Tokyo: Tokyo University of Foreign Studies.

Paul, Ileana. 2001. Concealed pseudo-clefts. Lingua 111(10). 707–727. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(00)00044-9

Payne, Doris & Immanuel Barshi (eds.). 1999. External possession 39. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.39.03pay

Pearson, Matthew. 2001. The clause structure of Malagasy: A Minimalist approach. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Los Angeles dissertation.

Pearson, Matthew. 2005. The Malagasy subject/topic as an A′-element. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 23(2). 381–457. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-004-1582-7

Perlmutter, David M. & Paul M. Postal. 1983. The relational succession law. In David M. Perlmutter (ed.), Studies in Relational Grammar 1. 30–80. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Pesetsky, David. 1987. Wh-in-situ: Movement and unselective binding. In Eric Reuland & Alice ter Meulen (eds.), The representation of (in)definiteness, 98–129. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Pylkkänen, Liina. 2008. Introducing arguments 49. Cambridge, MA: MIT press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262162548.001.0001

Rackowski, Andrea & Norvin Richards. 2005. Phase edge and extraction: A Tagalog case study. Linguistic Inquiry 36(4). 565–599. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1162/002438905774464368

Reintges, Chris H., Philip LeSourd & Sandra Chung. 2006. Movement, wh-agreement, and apparent wh-in-situ. In Lisa L.S. Cheng & Norbert Corver (eds.), Wh-movement: Moving on 42. 165–194. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Richards, Norvin. 1999. Featural cyclicity and the ordering of multiple specifiers. Current Studies in Linguistics 32. 127–158.

Ross, John Robert. 1986. Infinite syntax. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

Saddy, Douglas. 1991. Wh scope mechanisms in Bahasa Indonesia. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 15. 183–218.

Sato, Yosuke. 2008. Minimalist interfaces: Selected issues in Indonesian and Javanese. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona dissertation.

Sato, Yosuke. 2012. Successive cyclicity at the syntax-morphology interface: Evidence from standard Indonesian and Kendal Javanese. Studia Linguistica 66(1). 32–57. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9582.2011.01185.x

Sneddon, James Neil. 1996. Indonesian: A comprehensive grammar. New York: Routledge.

Sneddon, James Neil. 2006. Colloquial Jakartan Indonesian 581. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, The Australian National University.

Sneddon, James Neil, K. Alexander Adelaar, Dwi Noverini Djenar & Michael Ewing. 2012. Indonesian: A comprehensive grammar. New York: Routledge.

Stevens, Alan. 1968. Madurese Phonology and Morphology 52. New Haven: American Oriental Society.

Suharno, Ignatius. 1982. A descriptive study of Javanese 45. Canberra: Department of Linguistics, Research School of Pacific Studies, Australian National University.

Szabolcsi, Anna. 1992. The noun phrase. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Los Angeles dissertation.

Tuller, Laurice A.. 1986. Bijective relations in Universal Grammar. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Los Angeles dissertation.

Uhlenbeck, Eugenius M. 1978. Studies in Javanese morphology 19. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.

van Urk, Coppe & Norvin Richards. 2015. Two components of long-distance extraction: Successive cyclicity in Dinka. Linguistic Inquiry 46(1). 113–155. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1162/LING_a_00177

Voskuil, Jan. 1996. Comparative morphology: Verb taxonomy in Indonesian, Tagalog and Dutch 21. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.

Voskuil, Jan. 2000. Indonesian voice and A-bar movement. In Ileana Paul, Vivianne Phillips & Lisa Travis (eds.), Formal issues in Austronesian linguistics, 195–214. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.

Watanabe, Akira. 1996. Case absorption and wh-agreement. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8615-3

Wolff, John U., Didi Oetomo & Daniel Fietkiewicz. 1992. Beginning Indonesian through self-instruction 3. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University, Southeast Asia Program.

Yanti. 2010. A reference grammar of Jambi Malay. Newark, DE: University of Delaware dissertation.

Zaenen, Annie. 1983. On syntactic binding. Linguistic Inquiry 14(3). 469–504.