1 Introduction
Emojis (often bare plural emoji (OED 2023a)) are a relatively new form of visual communication that exist currently as an option on most digital keyboards. They are symbols that depict objects, signs, ideas, and facial expressions. People from all backgrounds and in various languages use emojis in daily computer-mediated communication (CMC), and to varying degrees. Use of emojis is now so frequent that they can be seen in advertisements (Knudson 2017), in print (Evans 2017), on clothing (WOWPresents 2015), and even personified in film (Leondis 2017).
Emojis have been researched by computer scientists, psychologists, sociologists, and linguists (Bai et al. 2019). The majority of linguistic research on emojis has focused on the semantics. This research follows the recent trend of asking the questions how can extralinguistic information contribute to the information conveyed in language? and can nonlinguistic meaning be modeled with tools form formal semantics? This contributes to the growing body of literature on so-called super semantics (Schlenker 2019). Yet, emojis bring up a multitude of interesting linguistic questions. The sociolinguistic use of emojis has been documented (Moschini 2016), and some have asked whether or not emojis have a syntax (Cohn & Engelen & Schilperoord 2019).
The focus of this paper, however, is the morphosyntactic properties of emojis that appear as elements inside of sentences, exemplified by the following well-known sentence.1
- (1)
- I NY
This example may seem straightforward and relatively uninteresting, but there is a wealth of data from Twitter and other websites that show very interesting morphosyntactic properties of emojis that appear as grammatical elements. The data is confirmed by the judgments of language users who are proficient with emojis. From these data and their analysis I show that these grammatical emojis represent predictable and cohesive morphological units that can participate in morphosyntactic processes such as inflection, derivation, lexicalization, and grammaticalization. Following previous work that discusses the possibility of non-linguistic units functioning as lexical elements (Wiese 1996), I even raise the question of whether some of these emoji words are uniquely represented in language users’ lexicons as independent units.
The paper is structured as follows: in Section 2 I outline a brief history of emojis and their use. In Section 3 I describe three functions that emojis can have in a written utterance. In Section 4 I expand in detail upon the facts surrounding the distribution of emojis that appear within sentences instead of words in three languages: English (4.1.1.), German (4.1.2.), and Spanish (4.1.3.). I discuss data of these emojis taking inflectional (4.1.) and derivational (4.2.) affixes. I then discuss morphological regularization (4.3.) and summarize the findings (4.4.). In Section 5 I show that these emojis are sensitive to processes of morphosyntactic change such as lexicalization (5.1.) and grammaticalization (5.2.). In Section 6 I discuss a possible two-way classification of these emojis based on how they represent lexical elements. Section 7 concludes.
2 Some Background on Emojis
The first emoji-like elements (in-text ideographic pictures not composed of preexisting characters) emerged in Japan in the late 1990s to convey the meanings of facial expressions seen in manga (Danesi 2017). These symbols eventually evolved into what we know as emojis. The word emoji comes from Japanese 絵 e ‘picture’ and 文字 moji ‘character’, and was first introduced by emoji inventor Shigetaka Kurita in 1999 (Evans 2017). The resemblance this word bears to English emoticon is pure coincidence, as emoticon is a portmanteau of emotion and icon that is attested since the mid-1980s. The emoji keyboard was introduced for Apple digital keyboards on the iPhone in 2008 with iOS 2.2 and is now a standard feature on almost all smartphones, tablets, and computers (Evans 2017).
Using ideograms in writing to convey emotion is no new feature of written language, though. The first attested smiling face – or smiley – in a text is found in a financial record in the town of Trenčín, Slovakia next to the author’s signature (Ladislaides, 1635). The symbol was used identically to one of the ways in which contemporary emojis are used, to indicate the author’s positive feelings about the preceding text (Votruba 2018).
The first recorded use of the emoticons :-) and :-( in CMC, a predecessor to emojis that are still frequently used, occurred in 1982 in a computer forum (Fahlman 1982). Though some have proposed that the expressive symbol consisting of a colon followed by a closed parenthesis is attested since the seventeenth century (Stahl 2014), these instances are more than likely coincidental.
The original Apple emoji keyboard contained 471 emojis, but there are 4,084 at the time of writing this paper. Each emoji has its own Unicode entry, keywords, and a short name (“grinning face”, “squid”, “flag of Australia”, etc.), which I will be subsequently referring to as emoji’s name or label. The original was comprised of 32 smileys, a number of pictograms and ideograms depicting various animals, objects, and abstract symbols, as well as the flags of ten countries. The current emoji keyboard contains over 100 smileys, over 100 pictograms of humans and human body parts in six different skin colors, hundreds upon hundreds of miscellaneous pictograms and ideograms, flags for every country in the world, and flags for the gay and trans communities (Emojipedia). New emojis are added often.
3 Functions of Emojis in Sentences
Emojis have multiple possible configurations of where they can go in a sentence, and these positions correspond to different linguistic functions. In this section I identify three broad categories of where emojis can go. I borrow terminology from the domain of super linguistics and the body of research on the semantics of gestures (Schlenker 2018) to name the categories post-text emojis, emojis that follow an utterance (3.1.), co-text emojis, emojis that immediately follow and are directly associated with one specific word or constituent embedded in a sentence (3.2.), and pro-text emojis, which are emojis that are actually syntactically projected in the sentence, and will be the main focus of the morphological analysis of this paper (3.3.). These terms have been used before as they relate to emojis (Pierini 2021; Maier 2023).
3.1 Post-text emojis
The vast majority of the current literature on formal linguistics of emojis is about the semantics of sentences containing post-text emojis (Gawne & McCulloch 2019; Grosz & Kaiser & Pierini 2021; Pierini 2021; Pasternak & Tieu 2022; Maier 2023), in which the semantics of these emojis are often analyzes in the same way as the semantics of gestures that follow an utterance. Within post-text emojis, Grosz & Kaiser & Pierini (2021) distinguish between two types: face emojis (2–3) and activity emojis (4).
- (2)
- a.
- Did you see that guy? (Grosz et al. 2022)
- b.
- That fried chicken sandwich they make
- c.
- If a movie is violent, Alex hates it
These face emojis in (2) are expressive elements that provide information on the attitudes that the speaker feels toward the proposition in the text. For example, the “smiling face with heart-eyes” emoji in (2a) indicates that the speaker feels infatuation about the aforementioned guy. In (2c), the “worried face” emoji expresses the speaker’s negative emotions about Alex’s hatred of violent movies. The examples in (2) are all dependent, which means that the interpretation in the text influences the interpretation of the emoji since the emoji comments on the proposition of the text, and this is the focus of Grosz et al. (2022), but there are examples where the emoji is independent as it offers no comment on the speaker’s attitude toward the text. See these below.
- (3)
- a.
- How did the interview go? (Grosz et al. 2022)
- b.
- How are you coping?
These examples are the most clearly similar to gestures and facial expressions in terms of how their meaning contributes to the sentence. The activity emojis in (4), while also appearing after the sentence, serve a different function. For Grosz, Kaiser, & Pierini (2021) these are event descriptions.
- (4)
- a.
- Arsenal really impressed me ! (Grosz & Kaiser & Pierini 2021)
- b.
- Getting ready for tomorrow!
- c.
- My job is pretty fun
Post-text emojis do not represent syntactic constituents, and it would be erroneous to suggest that they are projected in the syntax of the sentences they modify, just as no one suggests that post-speech gestures are projected syntactically. This is not to say, however, that no emojis can be represented in the syntax of a written utterance.
3.2 Co-text emojis
There are also co-text emojis, where emojis directly follow (often without spacing) a word or phrase that they modify.
- (5)
- I can build a house rebuild a car and dig your grave!!!
- (6)
- Breathe on me while I sleep tonight Lord!
- (7)
- Trans people are human!!
- (8)
- drink like Peter // hate like Stewie // be fly like Quagmire // roll like Joe
These emojis are comparable to co-speech gestures, which occur during an utterance and are associated with a specific constituent in speech (Esipova 2019; Hunter 2019). Co-text emojis and post-text emojis may be difficult to distinguish from one another when the word/phrase modified by the emoji is utterance-final, such as the , , and emojis in example (8). More work will be needed to differentiate these cases. To me it seems that co-text emojis iconically enrich syntactic constituents smaller than the level of an utterance, so the function of the emojis in (5) could be analyzed the same way as the activity emojis in (4).
I believe there is much interesting work to be done on co-text emojis, but this paper focuses on the morphology of the class of emojis described in the following section.
3.3 Pro-text emojis
The third class of emojis, pro-text emojis, are syntactically projected within a written utterance. These are so named for pro-speech gestures, which are also iconic elements that are syntactically projected within an utterance (Schlenker 2018). They are incredibly frequent across CMC in a variety of languages2, and are even commonly seen in some forms outside of electronic communication.
- (9)
- I [int: love; beer]3
Several researchers have pointed out instances in which emojis appear as parts of speech within as sentence (Al-Rashdi 2018; Cohn et al. 2018; Pierini 2021). Pierini (2021) calls these at-issue emojis, and compares them to Schlenker’s typology of pro-speech gestures. While some work suggests that these emojis replace written words (Tieu et al. 2023), the facts I show in this paper suggest a more interesting morphological process. Take the following example from Pierini (2021).
- (10)
- She is the [int: bomb] (Pierini 2021)
In this example, if the emoji is understood to be a replacement on an orthographic level for the fully formed English word ‹bomb›, then the possibility of morphosyntactic processes such as affixation happening with pro-text emojis would be surprising.
The central argument of this paper, to be discussed and analyzed in detail in the remaining sections, is that pro-text do not replace words at all, but in fact represent free stems. I do this by demonstrating that, across a variety of languages, emojis can take affixes, which would not be expected if the emojis are merely replacing words on an orthographic level. Furthermore, these affixes often times do not correspond to the morphology of the word that is associated with an emoji. I then show that emojis can appear in cases where there is no clear equivalent in spoken language at all, and that emojis can undergo morphosyntactic processes such as lexicalization and grammaticalization. Additionally, I show that pro-text emojis need not be associated with covert spoken words, which is something Schlenker (2017: 38) claims of pro-speech gestures as well. Experimental evidence (Cohn et al. 2018; Tieu et al. 2023) suggests that pro-text emojis can behave and be interpreted as other stems are.
The use of pro-text emojis has several potential factors, including but not limited to abbreviation, iconic enrichment (Schlenker 2017), or simply stylistic choice of the speaker. While language users’ motivations in selecting a pro-text emoji over a preexisting word are certainly an interesting area worthy of more exploration, this choice has no effect on the morphological generalizations I explore in the following sections.
4 Morphology of Pro-text emojis
Here I introduce data showing pro-text emojis in various languages. In addition to showing emojis as grammatical elements, I also show them combining with different kind of affixes. The restrictions on affixations are crucial to the analysis here. Discussion of what this means for the morphological categorization of these emojis follows.
All of the data, unless noted otherwise, come from Twitter4. I collected these data using Twitter’s search tool, which allows users to search for a specific token (in this case, an emoji) and filter by several different factors, the most relevant one here being language. A search that returns very few results I take to be poorly attested and a search that returns no results I consider completely unattested, and is thus preceded by an asterisk. A drawback of Twitter’s search tool is that it does not return specific numbers of results, so if a search yields a volume of results that is too high to manually count, then the exact number of results remains unknown. It is possible to get more specific numbers using Twitter’s API tool, but since the acquisition of Twitter by Elon Musk in 2023 and several policy changes following that, the API tools are far more restricted and difficult to access. Another challenge is that, to my knowledge, no parsed corpus of language from Twitter considers the possibility that emojis may be grammatical elements. My examples – unless otherwise noted – are not isolated occurrences; rather, there are several pages worth of results. Any form of an emoji-affix combination with over 100 or so results I consider well-attested, and the grammatical examples I show are – unless otherwise noted – well-attested.
The notion that emojis combining with certain affixes can be grammatical or ungrammatical is further corroborated by the intuitions of native speakers who are proficient with emojis as a form of communication. Grammaticality judgments about the examples initially gathered from Twitter were collected both in-person and online from between 7 and 10 informants for non-English languages, and quite significantly more informants for the English examples (>25), elicited in both individual and group settings. I treat these judgments as any other types of grammaticality judgments are treated for syntactic theory (Collins 2019). Collecting grammaticality judgments about pro-text emojis – or any visual elements for that matter – seems to be a relatively new task that has not been done much elsewhere. As such, several considerations must be made clear.
It is not the case that every language user is proficient in using and understanding pro-text emojis to the same degree. While most English speakers would likely not have trouble understanding an example like (1), almost all of my informants over the age of 40 (and many who are younger as well) reported having no judgments about pro-text emojis taking affixes. Why some people have judgements on these data and others do not is certainly an issue of modality (Cohn & Schilperoord 2022). Just as gestures and spoken language constitute separate modalities, pictorial symbols such as – but of course not limited to – emojis constitute a separate modality from speech and written language (Kress 2009). Cohn & Schilperoord (2022) distinguish between three modalities for human communication: graphic, vocal, and bodily, with emojis falling into the graphic category, speech into the vocal category, and gestures into the bodily category.
It is simultaneously true that every language user has access to these modalities, but some people have much more developed and refined grammatical systems for certain modalities than others do. For example, virtually all humans use gestures in communication (Abner & Cooperrider & Goldin-Meadow 2015), yet sign language users gestures in a far more frequent, systematic, and granular way than non-signers do (Goldin-Meadow & Brentari 2017). Another example is that people who are not fully proficient in visual languages are still able to create and recognize basic shapes (Cohn 2012). As such, while most language users may be able to understand basic instantiations of pro-text emojis, it does not follow from that fact that every language user should have refined judgments about the acceptable distribution of pro-text emojis.
Cohn (2012, 2022) raises questions of acquisition of different modalities, which is relevant to explain who exactly can be said to be proficient with pro-text emojis. To acquire a visual communication system specific to CMC, engaging regularly in CMC is crucial. It is known that younger age groups tend to spend more time online (Scott et al. 2017; Perrin & Atske 2021). Additionally, younger people who have spent little to no time growing up in a world without regular access to CMC and digital keyboards are far more likely to be “natively” familiar with emojis in a way that older people are not. Indeed, many young people do not even remember a time in which their digital keyboard layouts did not have the capability of producing emojis. Therefore, age is taken to be a predictor of who is most likely to have reliable judgments about the grammaticality of pro-text emojis, as younger age is associated with more time spent online and greater familiarity with emojis. This is also reflected in the fact that younger people tend to use emojis in creative and innovative ways that people in older age brackets tend not to understand (Ishmael 2021).
This is to say that those who provided grammaticality judgments for pro-text emojis in this project are people who are native speakers of the language in question who regularly engage in CMC and who are highly familiar with using emojis. The author also qualifies as one such person for English. Interestingly, Romance language speakers who do not use emojis frequently all share strong judgments about the ungrammaticality of certain examples, which I explore further later on. Several individuals who do not identify as proficient emoji users were also asked for judgments, most of whom reported having none.
The examples in this paper are naturally occurring, are backed up by grammaticality judgments from a significant number of relevant language users, and are confirmed by being well-attested or not attested forms online.
4.1 Inflectional Affixation
Pro-text emojis appear where one would normally expect a word. Examples of these are numerous and easy to find in a multitude of languages and in several syntactic categories. Verbs in (11), nouns in (12), and adjectives in (13).
- (11)
- a.
- I TEXAS! [int: love]
- b.
- We you Prime Minister…[int: see]
- c.
- I’m gonna u on ur forehead u look 2 cute [int: kiss]
- d.
- I need to before I see the end of this game or I’ll be I missed it [int: sleep, sad]
- (12)
- a.
- My hair has gotten so long I feel like a [int: mermaid]
- b.
- is where the is! [int: home; heart]
- c.
- i play the & the [int: violin; guitar]
- d.
- joon’s eating cubes from his iced … [int: ice; coffee]
- (13)
- a.
- Pratik is always good at heart, he is a person [int: happy, good]
- b.
- This has made me chuckle I needed this after a day! [int: shit(ty)]
- c.
- Just glad my passport allows entry to New Zealand…. [int: Australian]
- d.
- Some people were discriminated against at protest grounds [int: gay, LGBT]
Examples such as these are, of course, not limited to English. See verbs (14) and nouns (15) in several languages below.
- (14)
- a.
- Я
- I love
- минское
- minsk-ADJ
- море
- sea
- и
- and
- мою
- my-ACC
- подружку
- girlfriend-ACC
- Леру (Russian)
- Lera-ACC
- ‘I love the Minsk sea and my girlfriend Lera’
- b.
- Al
- to-the
- rato
- while
- vamos
- go-1.pl
- a
- to
- see
- a
- to
- militares
- soldiers
- dando
- giving
- clases
- classes
- en
- in
- kinder (Spanish)
- kindergarten
- ‘Pretty soon we’ll see soldiers teaching Kindergarten classes’
- c.
- Agora
- now
- eu
- i
- fly
- pelo
- for-the
- mundão (Brazilian Portuguese)
- world
- ‘Now I’ll fly around the world’
- (15)
- a.
- Les
- The
- elephant
- de
- of
- la.
- the
- colère !
- anger!
- Laissez
- leave-imp
- nous
- us
- en
- in
- paix
- peace
- vous
- you
- humains ! (French)
- humans !
- ‘Angry elephant ! Leave us in peace humans !
- b.
- Der
- the
- football
- rollt! (German)
- rolls
- ‘The football rolls!’
- c.
- el
- the
- pinchazo
- thorn-AUG
- en
- in
- el
- the
- heart
- cuando
- when
- te
- you
- dicen
- say-3.pl
- algo
- something
- que
- that
- temías (Spanish)
- fear-2.sg.ipfv
- ‘The thorn in the heart when someone tells you something were afraid to hear’
It is abundantly clear from Twitter data alone that emojis can represent elements that are projected in the syntax of a sentence, and this is evident in many languages in the world. But is it appropriate to simply say that emojis represent words? The following data show that emojis can take affixes, which suggests that emojis represent something smaller than what is conventionally referred to as a word. If emojis simply replace a word at the level of a sentence, then affixation should not be possible, but it is. Therefore more analysis is needed.
4.1.1 English Inflectional Affixes
See emojis in English taking inflectional – or grammatical – affixes for third person singular present agreement (16), past tense inflectional morphology (17), present progressive inflection (18), and participial inflection (19). All English inflectional affixes are well-attested with pro-text emojis.
- (16)
- a.
- He s to [int: loves; read]
- b.
- If in bed and she ’s his face while we do the nasty….I’m cool wit dat! [int: looks at]5
- c.
- But you must walk him often and he ’s his diaper a lot. [int: poops]
- (17)
- a.
- My therapist ed me so I took selfies in the parking lot [int: ghosted]
- b.
- IMO we ed better without him. [int: looked]
- c.
- … d each other like brothers… [int: loved]
- (18)
- a.
- fuck disney plus im ing [int: pirating]
- b.
- likeeee the secondhand embarrassment is ing meee [int: killing]
- c.
- …you may Think they ing marathon to win gold… [int: running]
- (19)
- a.
- This was someone else’s twt, but I would have d it had it been yours [int: loved]
- b.
- This world has ed my soul with its pain, asking for its return in code [int: kissed]
- c.
- We look forward to seeing what gets you ’ed up next. [int: fired up]
English also allows the pluralization of nominal emojis.
- (20)
- a.
- The way ’s act like it was so long ago when its just in their backyard [int: whites]
- b.
- Good luck to all our s swimming today [int: dolphins]
- c.
- a couple of s smoking s [int: fags; fags]
Comparative and superlative affixes also appear on English adjectival emojis.
- (21)
- a.
- Bitch say it to my face I’ll drop u er than 4 o’clock [int: deader]
- b.
- This is the est scene ever created! I’m dying!!!!! [int: whitest]
- c.
- er than the price of gas [int: higher]
The main generalization from these English data are clear: pro-text emojis can appear with affixes in English. Before making more meaningful claims about this, though, it is necessary to look at data from other languages with different morphology. I use German and Spanish here.
4.1.2 German Inflectional Affixes
These processes are not only possible in English. Take the following German examples showing verbal affixation (22), pluralization of nouns (23), diminutive affixes on nouns (24), and comparative/superlative morphology on adjectives (25).
- (22)
- a.
- Und
- and
- dann
- then
- kommen
- come.3pl
- erst
- {for
- mal
- now}
- 3
- 3
- Tweets,
- tweets
- die
- that
- Du
- you
- get
- PTCP-love-PST
- hast.
- have.2sg.pst
- ‘And then there’s the three tweets that you’ve liked’
- b.
- Mehr
- more
- so
- so
- vornerum
- in.front.of.around
- ge
- PTCP-eye.v
- ‘More like eyeing around in front’
- c.
- Wir
- we
- en
- block-1.pl.pres
- alle!
- all!
- ‘We block everyone!’
- (23)
- a.
- Vielen
- many
- lieben
- fond
- Dank
- thank
- fur
- for
- die
- the
- e,
- star-PL
- liebe
- love
- ‘Many thanks for the stars, love’
- b.
- Ich
- I
- geb zu,
- {admit}
- ich
- I
- habe
- have
- gehamstert
- hoarded
- alles
- everything
- für
- for
- die
- the
- n
- cat-PL
- ‘I admit, I’ve been hoarding – (I’d do) everything for the cats.’
- c.
- Gewitter
- thunderstorm
- n,
- goat-pl,
- n,
- cat-pl,
- e,
- pig-pl,
- blaue
- blue
- Kartoffeln
- potatoes
- un
- and
- Scheekönige.
- snow.kings.
- Biz
- Until
- jetzt
- now
- super!
- great
- ‘A thunderstorm, goats, cats, pigs, blue potatoes and snow kings. So far all is great!’
- (24)
- a.
- Guten
- good
- Morgen
- morning
- meine
- my
- chen
- dear-DIM
- ‘Good morning my dear ones’
- b.
- Wünsche
- wish.1sg.prs
- dir
- you.dat
- heute
- today
- luck-music-love
- und
- and
- immer
- always
- ein
- a
- lecker
- tasty
- chen
- coffee-DIM
- ‘Today, I wish you luck, music, love, and that you may always have nice coffee’
- c.
- Wer
- who
- bis
- until
- hierher
- here
- gelesen
- read.ptcp
- hat:
- has:
- ich
- I
- mag
- like
- dich.
- you.acc.
- Kriegst
- get.2sg.prs
- ein
- a
- chen
- little-star-DIM
- ‘(To) those who have read all through here: I like you, you get a star”
- (25)
- a.
- Ihr
- y’all
- seid
- are
- unsere
- our
- sten
- beloved-SPRL
- Menschen.
- humans
- ‘You are our most beloved people’
- b.
- sten
- heartfelt-SPRL
- Dank
- thank
- liebe
- dear
- Alexandra,
- Alexandra,
- Selbiges
- the.same
- wünsche
- wish.1sg.prs
- ich
- I
- dir
- you.dat
- auch
- also
- ‘My most heartfelt thanks, dear Alexandra, I wish the same to you’
It seems, then, that German pro-text emojis can take the same kinds of inflectional affixes as English emojis, including the diminutive suffix. In the German examples we can see cases of more robust agreement systems than English involving verbal agreement markers and plural markers on nouns that change based on Case. This is not surprising since German has a much larger inventory of inflectional affixes than English does.
Despite differences in the number of available inflectional affixes, English and German share many similarities in their inflectional systems. English inflectional morphology is always done to an uninflected base form that itself can stand alone as a word (or a ‘free form’ as described in Bloomfield (1933)). The same is true of German nouns and adjectives, but not necessarily of verbs (Kastovsky 1994). This means that for English inflectional stems and for German nominal and adjectival inflectional stems, there is an unmarked base form that also corresponds to a possible freestanding word. For German verbs, however, there is no unmarked base form. This means that German verbal stems need not correspond to a possible freestanding word.
Kastovsky (1994) describes the inflectional system of German nouns and adjectives and of English as a whole as word-based (Sapir 1925), since the inflectional stems can function as standalone words. He contrasts this with a stem-based system, such as the one for German verbs, in which the base form cannot function as a freestanding word. Before continuing with this discussion of morphological levels, it is necessary to look at data from another, non-Germanic language.
4.1.3 Spanish Inflectional Affixes
Pro-text emojis in Spanish can be seen taking a variety of inflectional affixes as well. Plural suffixes are well-attested for nouns (26) and adjectives (27).
- (26)
- a.
- Que
- that
- los
- the-masc.pl
- s
- dog-pl
- sigan
- continue-3.pl.sbjv
- ladrando
- stealing
- ‘May the dogs keep on stealing’
- b.
- Si
- if
- captas
- capture-2.sg
- o
- or
- saco
- take-1.sg
- las
- the-fem.pl
- s
- pear-pl
- y
- and
- las
- the-fem.pl
- s.
- apple-pl
- ‘If you capture or I take out the pears and the apples’
- c.
- Unos
- some
- s
- coffee-pl
- para
- for
- el
- the
- frío
- cold
- y
- and
- sueño
- dream
- ‘Some coffees for the cold and tiredness’
- (27)
- Podría
- can-1.sg.cond
- comer
- eat
- chilaquiles
- chilaquiles
- todos
- all-masc.pl
- los
- the-masc.pl
- s
- dog-pl
- días.
- days.
- ‘I could eat chilaquiles every damn day’
The Spanish diminutive suffix (28) and augmentative suffix (30) can also be seen with pro-text emoji nouns. Interestingly, the diminutive suffix seems sensitive to the phonological information of the stem as both the allomorphs -ito and -cito are attested. Though, interestingly, many emojis that can take -cito as a diminutive suffix can also take -ito (compare (28b,c) to (29)), which is evidence that pro-text emojis are not inherently specified for phonological form.
- (28)
- a.
- Solito
- alone-DIM
- como
- like
- un
- a
- ito
- dog-DIM
- me
- me
- dejarom
- left-3.pl
- ‘They left me alone like a puppy dog’
- b.
- Alto
- high
- finde,
- weekend
- el
- the-masc.sg
- cito
- heart-DIM
- lleno
- full
- de
- of
- amorrr
- love
- ‘Great weekend, my heart is full of love’
- c.
- Fan
- fan
- del
- of-the
- cito
- sun-DIM
- de
- of
- otoño-invierno
- autumn-winter
- ‘Fan of the little sun of autumn-winter’
- (29)
- a.
- Amo
- love-1.sg
- a
- to
- mi
- my
- novio
- boyfriend
- con
- with
- todo
- all
- mi
- my
- ito
- heart-DIM
- ‘I love my boyfriend with all my heart-DIM’
- b.
- Un
- one
- día
- day
- más
- more
- sin
- without
- el
- the
- ito
- sun-DIM
- ‘One more day without the sun-DIM’
- (29)
- a.
- Así
- so
- me
- me
- pone
- put-3.sg
- el
- the
- dog
- azo
- sun-AUG
- ‘The fucking sun makes me like this’
- b.
- Otro
- other
- AZO
- goal-AUG
- de
- of
- Ángel
- Angel
- Robles
- Robles
- y
- and
- goal
- de
- of
- Guillermo
- Guillermo
- Martinez
- Martinez
- ‘Another big goal for Angel Robles and a goal for Guillermo Martinez’
- c.
- No
- no
- pues
- then
- gracias
- thanks
- por
- for
- el
- the-masc.sg
- azo
- plane-AUG
- monumental!
- monumental
- ‘No thanks for the monumental disaster!’
According to Twitter data and the intuitions of native Spanish speakers, these are the only inflectional affixes with pro-text emojis that are possible in Spanish. This notably leaves out gender suffixes on nominals and verbal agreement/tense suffixes, which is important for this analysis.
Observe the following examples, which are confirmed ungrammatical by a significant number of native Spanish speakers who are proficient emoji users. Additionally, Twitter searches for emojis followed by verbal phi-feature and tense agreement markers (and infinitival suffixes) and those followed by nominal/adjectival gender suffixes yield zero results. As such, I confidently label these examples ungrammatical due to not being attested at all and also being ubiquitously rejected by all native speakers questioned.
- (31)
- a.
- *Yo
- I
- o
- love-1.sg
- a
- to
- mi
- my
- novio
- boyfriend
- [int: yo amo a mi novio]
- ‘I love my boyfriend’
- b.
- *Quiero
- want-1.sg
- ar
- watch-INF
- la
- the
- tele
- TV
- [int: quiero mirar la tele]
- ‘I want to watch TV’
- c.
- *Nos
- refl
- ábamos
- kiss-1.sg.ipfv
- [int: nos besábamos]
- ‘We kissed each other’
- (32)
- a.
- *Todos
- all the
- los os
- dog-m.pl
- días
- days
- [int: todos los perros días]
- ‘Every fucking day’
- b.
- *Toda
- all the
- la a
- dog-f.sg
- gente
- people
- [int: toda la perra gente]
- ‘All of the fucking people’
With this additional data showing what is and is not possible with pro-text emojis in Spanish, the task of coming up with a morphological theory of these emojis becomes much clearer. I make sense of these examples in the following subsection.
4.1.4 Emojis as Stems
At this point it is clear that pro-text emojis do not merely replace words in a sentence after the derivation has been completed: they represent something smaller than a word that actually is part of the morphosyntactic construction of the sentence. Since emojis take affixes, they must represent something below the level of a word in a hierarchy of morphological levels that is something akin to the following (Lieber 1976; Selkirk 1982; Olsen 1986):
- (33)
- Words > stems > roots
I take ‘word’ here to mean any freestanding form of a given syntactic category with any acceptable number of inflectional and derivational affixes. Words are typically independently meaningful and often the target of syntactic operations such as movement. Stems, the level below words, are a lexically-typed base to which inflectional affixation is done. Stems may themselves contain affixes and their forms may or may not correspond with an acceptable freestanding words. Roots are bare elements containing zero affixes of any kind and often do not correspond to any freestanding form. There is much variation of these definitions in the literature (Kiparsky 2020; Lohndal 2020). These notions – especially that of a stem – are very important to this analysis and will be explored in detail in this section and following sections.
Stems are specified for a given syntactic category and are the target of affixation (Aronoff 1994). Notions of different types of morphological stems will be useful in determining what exactly pro-text emojis can represent, morphosyntactically speaking. The idea of the level-ordering hypothesis in morphology (Siegel 1974; Giegerich 1999; 2005; Kiparsky 2015; Bermúdez-Otero 2018) echoes what came up in Section 4.1.2. from Kastovsky (1994) about word-based and stem-based morphology by distinguishing between word-level and stem-level affixes.
To reiterate, all English inflectional affixes and all German nominal/adjectival inflectional affixes are word-based, while all German verbal affixes are stem-based. The terminology ‘word’ and ‘stem’ starts to get confusing as there are several slightly-competing definitions of these concepts floating around in the literature. Here I clarify that stem-level affixes promote roots to lexically-typed free stems, and word-level affixes promote lexically-typed stems (which may or many not already be able to function as a freestanding word6) to inflected freestanding words. Therefore, the names ‘word-level’ and ‘stem-level’ refer to the output of the affixation, not the input. I now give an example of the representation of morphological structure assumed here on out. Roots are the lowest level that combine with an affix, that affix being a word-level affix. Intermediate stem levels combine with word-level affixes from which they are either promoted to words or become a stem for more affixes. The highest node represents a freestanding word.
- (34)
- Morphological structure of words
Before getting into pro-text emoji examples, take the English word nations as an example of these levels. Here we have the root √NAT, the derivational stem-level suffix -ion, and the inflectional word-level suffix -s.
- (35)
- Structure of English nations
The root √NAT must take some form of derivational morphology (here, the nominalizing suffix -ion to be promoted to a lexically typed (meaning that it is specified for a given syntactic category) stem, otherwise it is not a candidate for inflectional affixation, hence the ungrammaticality of a form like *nats with this root. Some English roots, such as √OPT may be promoted to a lexically typed stem via a null derivational morpheme.
With these generalizations in mind, let us take a grammatical example of a pro-text emoji such as s, meaning apples [N, pl]. I am assuming in the morphological and syntactic derivations that these pro-text emojis directly represent the stems merged into these positions, as opposed to replacing something else at a later point in the derivation of the utterance.
- (36)
- Structure of English s
We can contrast this with an unattested form such as *al,7 meaning floral [Adj], the structure of which is given below. In addition to being unattested, this form is also judged unacceptable by English speaking emoji users, especially in contrast to an example like s above.
- (37)
- Structure of English *al
- *
This contrasts suggests that emojis are unavailable as roots and must instead represent lexically-typed stems, since pro-text emojis are not able to appear taking a stem-level affix that combines directly with a root. As further evidence of this, I introduce the differences between two types of Spanish nominal suffixes: gender and number suffixes.
Gender suffixes are always closest to the root in Spanish, and number always follows gender. Take the Spanish example perros ‘dogs’, with the base form √PERR followed by the masculine suffix -o and the plural suffix -s. The order ROOT-GEN-NUM is the only possible configuration of these morphemes, and no other combination is possible (*perrso is completely impossible). This means that gender is undeniably closer to the root than number, but how close exactly is a matter of some contention. The first possibility is that gender is an inflectional suffix that attaches to nominal stems (Picallo 1991; Alexiadou 2004). See perros with this internal structure, in which the root √PERR must be an available candidate for affixation. The second option, which is that gender features are actually located on the nominalizing head, means that the gender suffix itself is what promotes the root to a nominal stem. This analysis is explored in detail in Kramer (2016). For our purposes, this means that the gender suffix is directly adjoined to the root, creating a nominal stem that can be inflected.
- (38)
- Structure of Spanish perros with gender nominalizer
There is sufficient evidence to suggest emojis cannot represent roots, With (38), the gender suffix is what turns the root into a nominal, as the gender feature is located on the nominalizing head in the analysis from Kramer (2016). As the stem, not the root, is the target of inflectional affixation, this means that the smallest possible stem in the word perros does not correspond with the root √PERR, but is the root plus the nominalizing head -o: perro. In other words, the gender suffix is a stem-level suffix, and the number suffix is a word-level suffix. So, the internal structure of s would be as follows.
- (39)
- Structure of Spanish s
Since emojis are not available to represent roots, *os is ruled out as an acceptable form.
- (40)
- Structure of Spanish *os
- *
Assuming stems are what are stored in the language user’s lexicon (Anderson 1992; Zwicky 1992; Aronoff 1994), these bare root forms of Spanish nominals are not actually available as morphosyntactic primitives since they are not accessible directly from the lexicon. Stems, however, do contain this necessary morphosyntactic information. The gender suffixes of Spanish nouns and adjectives,, as they create a stem from the root, are the smallest unit in the lexicon of elements of these syntactic categories. Bermúdez-Otero (2013: 5) gives the following tree, slightly modified for the purpose of this paper, for the noun encuentros [masc, pl].
- (41)
- Structure of Spanish encuentros
In this structure, the stem is entered in lexicon, not anything lower. As this relates to pro-text emojis, this means that they cannot appear as nouns or adjectives in Spanish as anything smaller than what is entered in the lexicon – anything smaller than a stem.
This paradigm also explains the inability of Spanish verbal emojis to appear with inflectional affixes, as these inflectional affixes also combine directly with the root. The differences between English and Spanish verbal emojis and this proposed explanation here are in line with preexisting observations about English and Spanish verbal morphology. We would not expect some primitive Spanish verbal root without affixes to exist in the lexicon. According to Lasnik (1995), French verbs are fully inflected in the lexicon of French speakers, so every possible inflected form of a verb is stored in the speaker’s lexicon, which is related to the fact that there are no bare forms of verbs, with even the infinitive forms having a suffix (the same is true of Spanish). Lasnik also argues that all English forms are bare in the lexicon, with verbal affixes being inserted in the syntax. Indeed, the same contrast between affixation with English and Spanish verbal emojis also exists with English and French, with an example such as (42) being incredibly well-attested online and confirmed acceptable by multiple native speakers. In contrast, (43), and examples like it of a verbal inflectional affix containing phi and tense information attaching to a pro-text emoji are not attested whatsoever online and are universally rejected by French speakers.
- (42)
- Nous
- we
- love
- Paris
- Paris
- ‘We love Paris’
- (43)
- *Nous
- we
- ons
- love-1.pl
- Paris
- Paris
- ‘We love Paris’
This analysis of French verbal morphology can indeed be extended to Spanish, as Spanish verbs are also stored in the lexicon this way as inflected stems. For the Spanish verb amar (love), there is no root form in the lexicon am- without the infinitival suffix. Rather, there are lexical entries for every possible verb form including the infinitive and all inflectional combinations of person, number, and tense (am-o, am-as, am-a, am-amos, etc.). This unavailability of the bare form as a lexical stem means that the inflectional affixes are inseparable from the root on a lexical level. Since there is no bare verbal form available in Spanish, an emoji in the following position is not possible.
- (44)
- Structure of Spanish *o (love-1.sg) [int: amo]
- *
Spanish verbal emojis cannot appear with affixes because there exists no level in the lexicon at which the inflected Spanish verbal stems are separable from their roots. This is different from English, where verbs are stored in the lexicon as bare stems, hence the grammaticality of (16–19). Spanish gender suffixes for nouns and adjectives, as well as Spanish verbal inflectional affixes, form part of the basic elements of the lexicon; they are stem-level affixes. All of the examples thus far demonstrate that emojis appear in positions of lexically-typed stems.
Saying that pro-text emojis are available as roots would vastly overpredict the places where we see them and the types of affixes that we see them combine with. Looking at the affixes that pro-text emojis do and do not combine with in different languages, it is clear that pro-text emojis prefer taking word-level affixes. As such, across the languages discussed so far, it is clear that the distribution of pro-text emojis corresponds perfectly with other elements that are known to be stems, not roots, so it is safe to say that pro-text emojis represent stems. Preliminary data suggest that this is also the case in Slavic languages such as Russian (Thelin 1973), where forms like (45a) are attested and used by native speakers, whereas forms like (45b) are not attested at all and judged unacceptable by native speakers.
- (45)
- a.
- Я
- I
- love
- тебя
- you.acc
- ‘I love you’
- b.
- *Я
- I
- лю/ю
- love-1.sg.pres
- тебя
- you.acc
- ‘I love you’
Perhaps the intuition that pro-text emojis are words is not so far-off, an intuition reflected by the Oxford Word of The Year in 2015, which was the emoji . The truth of this intuition is that pro-speech only combine with word-level affixes, not stem-level affixes. It is accurate to say, then, that pro-text emojis must represent something that can be lexically represented.
4.2 Derivational Affixation
Thus far we have only seen inflectional affixes. In English, all inflectional affixes are word-level affixes, but this clearly is not the case in a language such as Spanish. So, what happens in English with stem-level affixes? For this we must turn to derivational – or word changing – affixation. Many English speakers accept these examples, and there is no shortage of them online. Examples including but not limited to adjective forming -ly (46), adjective forming -y (47) nominalizing -ness (48), and adjective forming -ish (49) are abundant. Even the agentive -er suffix is attested (50).
- (46)
- Have a ly Day! [int: lovely]
- (47)
- a.
- so y boys [int: icy]
- b.
- now no one can say I am being dramatic when I call it a y day!!!! [int: shitty]
- c.
- Just y. [int: shitty]
- (48)
- a.
- Happy New Year Krystal all the love and ness to you [int: happiness]
- b.
- Looking at this picture, just brings me great ness [int: sadness]
- c.
- Some days I think I’ve reached my peak ’ness. [int: gayness]
- (49)
- a.
- I real feel ish [int: awkwardish?]
- b.
- This is just getting ish. [int: childish]
- (50)
- a.
- Most under rated -er [int: writer]
- b.
- Trump doesn’t see a problem with that because he is a 1st class er [int: ass-kisser]
- c.
- Every er should have a stash [int: smoker]
These examples are not to suggest that all derivational affixes are compatible with pro-text emojis. Take another unattested example that native English speakers proficient with emojis judge ungrammatical.
- (51)
- *ity killed the cat [int: Curiosity killed the cat]
Once again, the choice of emoji is not what makes this sentence ungrammatical. Language users surely could make endless arguments about what emoji better conveys curious better among the many possible emojis, but it is the position of the emoji here that makes it ungrammatical. No emoji is attested as being modified by the nominalizing suffix -ity, whereas there are abundant examples of emojis being modified by -ness. There even are examples of emojis with -ness that have a similar interpretation to curiousness, which are also judged to be better than (51) by emoji-proficient native speakers.
- (52)
- I feel like I remember him commenting on the -ness of her outfit [int: curiousness]
The difference between these cases is that -ity is a stem-level derivational affix, while -ness is a word-level derivational affix (Kiparsky 2020). This distinction between derivational affixes is also noted in Distributed Morphology (Marantz 2007; Embick & Marantz 2008). Here the difference is captured by the terms inner derivation and outer derivation. Inner derivation is the process of attaching derivational affixes to roots while outer derivation is the process of attaching derivational affixes to lexically-typed stems.
The difference between curiosity and curiousness, then, is that the -ity of curiosity attaches directly to the root √CURIOUS, whereas -ness in curiousness attaches to the adjectival stem curious. This distinction is also visible in the phonological and orthographic effects that these processes have on their final products, as -ity changes the stress and spelling of the root, whereas the root curious as it is pronounced in isolation is preserved in the word curiousness. Since pro-text emojis cannot be roots, ness is acceptable as curiousness, but *ity is not acceptable as curiosity. See the structures below.
- (53)
- Structure of English curiosity
- (54)
- Structure of English curiousness
- (55)
- Structure of English *ity
- (56)
- Structure of English ness
It is the case that inner layer derivational affixes may not appear after a pro-text emoji. This is completely expected under the assumption that emojis must represent lexically-typed stems, not roots. This assumption is generalized as the following condition.
- (57)
- The Pro-Text Emoji Condition
- Pro-text emojis must represent a stem that is specified for a given syntactic category.
I now turn to an apparent violation of the Pro-Text Emoji Condition. But first, the inability of a certain English affix to combine with pro-text emojis must be established.
- (58)
- a.
- *I only wear als in the spring [int: floral]
- b.
- *You look ar to me [int: familiar]
- c.
- *We have al insurance [int: dental]
- d.
- *Ready for the ar new year [int: lunar]
Based on these data, it seems as if emojis with the adjective-forming -al/-ar suffix are not permitted. The suffix -al (and its allomorphic variant -ar) is a stem-level suffix (Kiparsky 2020), so this is in line with the condition above. Yet, the following example somewhat attested (though I am not sure if I myself accept these as grammatical).
- (59)
- a.
- Operation Normal: Trump Goes -al [int: global]
- b.
- The to disrupting the al hegemony is controlling the inventory of laundromats
- c.
- We are currently at 420ppm CO2 & +1.1°C increase in al temp
I now turn to a few other examples in which emojis appear after what look like inner-layer derivational affixes.
- (60)
- a.
- Le Président…is going to avoid the term -ization from now [int: Finlandization]
- b.
- same Montreal street before/after ization [int: pedestrianization]
- c.
- TRUMP NK DEIZATION: FAILED [int: denuclearization]
- (61)
- a.
- the whole trump administration literally…ized it [int: weaponized]
- b.
- …as symptoms of the end-stage disease of ized society [int: atomized]
- c.
- We’re getting ized. [int: Islamized]
Examples (59)–(61) all show stem-level affixes (Kiparsky 2020) appearing after pro-text emojis. (59) shows adjective-forming -al, and (60) and (61) both show verb-forming -ize, with (60) having more derivational affixes stacked on top of it, and (61) showing an inflectional affix following it. While these examples may seem problematic, the fact is that these affixes can either be word-level and stem-level (Siegel 1974; Aronoff 1976; Selkirk 1982; Aronoff & Sridhar 1983). Giegerich (1999) refers to these as dual membership affixes, while Kiparsky (2020) calls them chameleon affixes. The affixes can attach to bound roots as well as words (Giegerich 1999: 13). This contrast is clear with the attested pro-text emoji examples, as well as the unattested examples.
The emoji in (59) clearly corresponds to the English free morpheme globe. Looking at (58), the emojis correspond to the bound roots flor- (a), famili- (b), dent- (c), and lun- (d). Similarly, the examples in (60) and (61) all correspond to free morphemes in English. Some of these are proper names such as Finland and Islam, which intuitively must be specified as nominal. Additionally some of these emojis correspond to words that contain category-specific morphology of some kind, i.e. -ar of nuclear and -ian of pedestrian. As such, it only makes sense to posit that these instances of -al and -ize are actually word-level affixes. An example of the stem-level suffix -ize would be a word such as militarize, in which the root is a bound morpheme of no specific syntactic category. Indeed, it is not possible to represent this root with an emoji, as something like *ize or *ize is not attested at all (on Twitter or Google) and admittedly very difficult to parse. This again lends credence to the condition that pro-text emojis can only occur as lexically-typed stems.
4.3 Morphological Irregularities
Circling back to inflectional morphology, there are inflectional forms in English that are irregular which can be regularized in the presence of a pro-text emoji. See the following examples.
- (62)
- a.
- counting s to fall asleep [int: sheep]
- b.
- Lemme tell you bout these 3 blind s [int: mice]
- c.
- Finna search for some bigger s for my s [int: diamond;, teeth]
Similarly, while the following examples are attested, there are no attested instances of *i or *i, which are degraded.
- (63)
- a.
- whos idea was it to put ’s at the headboard of a bed tf [int: cactuses]
- b.
- We need s & es in that case! [int: crabs, octopuses]
Irregular forms that involve ablaut or plural suffixes other than -s are not available for pro-text emojis as the emojis themselves are the stem. In other words, the stem has no vowels to be ablauted. For the irregular plural suffixes such as cacti, the derivation would involve stem-level affixation to the root as these forms come from Latin, which forms plurals via stem-level derivation, which we have already established that emojis cannot do.
4.4 Interim Summary
So far I have demonstrated, among the ways in which emojis are used in language, there is a distinct category of emojis functioning as grammatical elements, called pro-text emojis. Upon further examination of data from Twitter as well as the judgments of native speakers in English, German, Spanish (and French and Russian to some degree) it becomes clear that pro-text emojis felicitously and regularly are modified by both inflectional and derivational affixes. Analysis of the types of affixation possible across the aforementioned languages as well as of current morphological theory strongly suggests that pro-text emojis are only possible as stems of a specified syntactic category that take word-level affixes, which in English includes inflectional affixes and some derivational affixes. This generalization, summarized in The Pro-Text Emoji Condition of (57), has been reached not by operating under the assumption that pro-text emojis must work the same way in different languages, but by observing in multiple languages a similarity in the types of affixes that are permitted and not permitted with pro-text emojis.
There is an interesting cross-linguistic contrast from the data in this section that merits discussion as well, namely that the Romance ungrammatical examples are more disliked than the English ungrammatical examples. By this I mean that even the Spanish and French speakers who are not proficient in emoji use have strong intuitions that the ungrammatical examples of pro speech emojis are bad, while also accepting the grammatical ones. This is notably different from English speakers who are not proficient emoji users who often do not have strong judgments either way about these examples (though those who are proficient emoji users do have strong judgments). From the conversations I have had with some German speakers about this issue, German patterns more closely to English in this respect. This points to interesting differences in the morphological systems of English (or, Germanic, perhaps more broadly) and Romance as a whole. This builds on work by Lasnik (1995) and Uriagereka & Gallegos (2023) about the way that inflected verb forms are represented in the lexicon in English and Romance, respectively. According to these approaches,
Evidence from the regular inflectional morphology of pro-text emojis whose closest lexical equivalent inflects irregularly suggests that pro-text emojis can be morphologically totally distinct from the words whose meanings they correspond to. This does not necessarily mean, however, that pro-text emojis are always distinct lexical items on their own. It is important to keep in mind that emojis are first and foremost orthographic units. Just as it is not necessary to posit that eleven and 11 are not representations of distinct lexical items, it may not be necessary to posit that every pro-text emoji suggests a distinct lexical item from a preexisting word with a similar or identical meaning, but as seen in Section 4.3., they may not always be the same, either. As such, pro-text emojis, since they do not specify phonological content like traditional orthography does, may be ambiguous as to exactly what lexical item they refer to. Take the following examples of the same emoji referring to multiple lexical items (64) and different emojis referring to the same lexical item (65) as evidence of this.
- (64)
- a.
- I want to forever [int: sleep]
- b.
- This edible about to have me in my like… [int: bed]
- c.
- of lies [int: tired]
- (65)
- a.
- I want to forever [int: sleep]
- b.
- Don’t disturb me, I’m trying to [int: sleep]
- c.
- I on and off all day! [int: sleep]
With this ambiguity, it is possible to have several different types of lexical representation for pro-text emojis. In Section 5 I show that pro-text emojis can undergo processes of lexical change such as lexicalization and grammaticalization, and in Section 6 I discuss issues of iconicity and representation, showing that certain types of pro-text emojis may be directly represented in the lexicon.
5 Pro-text emojis and Language Change
As grammatical elements, it stands to reason that pro-text emojis are sensitive to processes of morphosyntactic change, which is exactly what we see. This means undergoing category changes, being borrowed between languages, novel forms being innovated, and the like. In this section I discuss pro-text emojis undergoing morphological processes of language change, namely lexicalization (Section 5.1.) and grammaticalization (Section 5.2.).
5.1 Lexicalization
In this section I discuss cases of emojis undergoing lexicalization, which here means that pro-text emojis become uniquely associated with a novel stem in the language user’s lexicon. I also compare these to processes of lexicalizing abbreviations, which is another common phenomenon in CMC, though not exclusive to CMC, just as the use of pictograms to represent grammatical elements can be found outside of CMC as well.
5.1.1 Novel Pro-Speech Emojis
It is quite common to see expressive emojis functioning as verbs.
- (66)
- a.
- Well you can see me ing but yeahhh i signed up for this. [int: facepalming]
- b.
- my brother caught me ing [int: just standing there]
To focus on a specific example, take as an expressive emoji.
- (67)
- a.
- He flew 40 hours from the UK to take me on our first date
- b.
- friendly reminder that Grogu got Din a father’s day gift
- c.
- Pisces moon got me wanting some love & affection
Now, observe the same emoji with verbal morphology. Here I leave out a translation of the emoji in brackets in the examples as I discuss it in the text.
- (68)
- a.
- Don’t at me, you little bottom
- b.
- never in my life have i ed at a girl like this
- c.
- can we all just take a second to at the show’s twitter bio
- d.
- are u ing other bitches
- e.
- I’m a top and I all the time stop this slander
What exactly this emoji means is hard to formalize. Many compare it to the expression ‘uwu’, but that is even more meaningless to those who are outside of communities who say that. Undoubtedly, is highly expressive as it clearly resembles a human facial expression. Emojipedia describes as a “face with furrowed eyebrows, a small frown, and large, ‘puppy dog’ eyes, as if begging or pleading. May also represent adoration or feeling touched by a loving gesture.” The website also mentions that it can be “somewhat suggestive” and even “horny” (Burge 2020). As a verb, then, means something along the lines of ‘to make the facial expression ’.
Also interesting is that there is no clear spoken English equivalent of this verb. This is unlike the following cases, where the emojis could be compared to ‘smile(v)’ and ‘laugh(v)’, respectively.
- (69)
- a.
- This made me saw a going nom nom while I was watching tv [int: smile; rabbit]
- b.
- over here ing at the Yankees [int: laughing]
This is independent evidence that this emoji does not replace a preexisting word, as there is no word that could replace. The data in this section show that the emoji is no longer just an expressive emoji, but is now a pro-text emoji that represents a stem that is not associated with another English lexical item. The emoji represents a novel lexical item in this way.
Another example comes from Brazilian Portuguese. In Brazil in the spring of 2022 a video of a lobster went viral online. In this video, the lobster can be seen walking up to and jumping in a pot of boiling oil.8 This video quickly became a meme that was shared in video and GIF form across social media. In the wake of this, the use of the emoji shown below appeared.
- (70)
- a.
- vou
- go-1.sg
- me
- self
- kill
- hj
- today
- 13h
- 13-hours
- ‘I’m going to kill myself today at 1:00pm’
- b.
- vc
- you
- se
- self
- kill
- ‘Kill yourself’
- c.
- Começando
- beginning
- o
- the
- feriadão
- holiday
- com
- with
- cólica
- colic
- vou
- go-1.sg
- me
- self
- kill
- ‘Starting the holiday with colic I’m going to kill myself’
Before this, the lobster emoji can be seen consistently as a pro-text emoji of the category Noun in Brazilian Portuguese tweets. A Twitter search for the emoji in Portuguese prior to 2022 shows no uses of the emoji as in (70).
- (71)
- a.
- Não
- No
- vai
- go-3.sg
- mais
- more
- comer
- eat
- lobster
- tão
- so
- cedo
- soon
- ‘Don’t go eating eating lobster so soon’
- b.
- Vontade
- wish
- de
- of
- comer
- eat
- lobster
- ‘Feel like eating lobster’
- c.
- Só
- only
- vou
- go-1.sg
- parar
- stop
- quando
- when
- acabar
- finish
- a
- the
- lobster
- e
- and
- wine
- ‘I’m only going to stop when the lobster and wine runs out’
In this case the pro-text emoji is going from representing a Noun to representing a Verb. One informant9 even states that they would pronounce the verbal emoji as lagostar, which is the nominal form lagosta plus the verbal infinitival suffix -ar, which is what would expected of a verb derived from a noun in Brazilian Portugese. Interestingly, though, the meaning is completely unpredictable from the form, quite unlike a case such as fish (N) and fish (V) in English. Here the meaning is dependent on the shared understanding of the meme among language users.
5.1.2 Borrowing of Pro-text emojis
One of the main processes of lexicalization is borrowing from other languages (Hilpert 2019). This can happen with pro-text emojis as well. While it may seem like the same pro-text emoji appearing in different languages is just an instance of two different lexical items in the same language (el in Spanish and ‘the ’ in English, for example), this is not always the case. There are verifiable instances of pro-text emojis being borrowed from one language to another.
Take the acronym G.O.A.T., which stands for ‘greatest of all time’ in English and is a very popular superlative on Twitter, especially used to describe athletes (Curtis 2017). Due to this acronym’s orthographic and phonological similarity to the noun ‘goat’, the emoji is often used as a predicative adjective as well. See the following example.
- (72)
- He is the but I need to see the batfleck in his own movie
This use of the emoji has been borrowed into Spanish as well, and is very commonly used by Spanish speakers on Twitter.
- (73)
- a.
- Garnacho
- Garnacho
- sabe
- knows
- quien
- who
- es
- is
- el
- the-masc.sg
- ‘Garnacho knows who the G.O.A.T. is’
- b.
- El
- the
- y
- and
- Messi
- Messi
- a
- to
- su
- his
- lado.
- side
- ‘The G.O.A.T. and Messi by his side’
The masculine determiner in these examples is important to note because the word for goat in Spanish is feminine (la cabra), so it is not the case that this is an instance of a pro-text emoji that corresponds to the Spanish word for goat being used in a novel way. Spanish pro-text emojis contain grammatical gender information while not needing an overt gender suffix, which makes sense considering the nature of gender morphology in Spanish, and this gender typically corresponds to the gender of the emoji’s closest synonym. While it may be possible that the emoji is being used in a way closer to a word such as cabrón ‘bastard’, which is masculine, the sentiment of the Spanish tweets containing is almost entirely positive and is used in the same sports contexts and it is in English.
5.1.3 Abbreviations
The lexicalization processes seen above with emojis are not new in any way; no new means of lexicalization need to be posited for pro-text emojis. They are immediately recognizable as similar to lexicalization of other elements, especially those commin in CMC.
Abbreviations in CMC such as lol ‘laugh out loud’ or lmao ‘laugh my ass off’ represent VPs, but they are able to be lexicalized as verbs that can take objects (74), or even nouns (75). (74b) is especially interesting when considering that the possessive pronoun ‘my’ is clearly no longer part of the composition of the verb.
- (74)
- a.
- im on harry styles baldtok im lmaoing
- b.
- my gf just lmao’d at a non-funny tweet.
- (75)
- a.
- Twitter should ban Elon Musk just for the lols
- b.
- Thank you for a mighty LOL. That thread is gold.
This is, of course, not specific to CMC, though. Similar processes lead to apparent redundancies such as ‘ATM machine’, ‘PIN number’, ‘NYI institute’, ‘BIPoC people’, and so forth.
Abbreviations also undergo borrowing between languages. Take the following examples of a French abbreviation in English, an English abbreviation in Spanish, and an English abbreviation in Russian.
- (76)
- So many ppl have already RSVP’d and I sent it 2 hours ago.
- (77)
- Pero
- but
- es
- is
- un
- a
- LOL
- LOL
- mayúsculo
- majuscule
- ‘But it’s a capital LOL’
- (78)
- имхо
- imho
- твиттер
- больше
- more
- всего
- everything-gen
- хейтят..
- hate-prs.pl..
- ‘IMHO Twitter is the most hated’
The interpretation of these abbreviations does not depend on understanding what it stands for in its original language. For example, English speakers regularly say ‘RSVP’ without necessarily knowing what it stands for in French. This resembles the example in English/Spanish.
5.2 Grammaticalization
If stems that are uniquely represented by pro-text emojis can be lexicalized, surely they can undergo grammaticalization as well. This would entail an emoji going from a lexical element to a functional element (Campbell & Janda 2000; Roberts & Roussou 2003). Since emojis can indeed be lexical items, such a thing should be possible, and indeed it is attested. Take the following examples, which are common among young people on internet communities such as ‘stan Twitter’ (Chung 2020) and ‘gay Twitter’ (Zulier 2021).
- (79)
- a.
- also we going doctor tomorrow
- b.
- Going Nevada to count these fucking ballots
- c.
- going supreme court y’all need anything?
- d.
- IM GOING MED SCHOOL
- e.
- going doctors for this mf migraine
Here the airplane emoji looks to correspond to the word ‘to’, indicating directionality and introducing the DPs that follow them. This emoji says nothing about the modality of travel, as in the emoji in (79a) does not contribute the meaning ‘we going to the doctor tomorrow by plane’. We also see the emoji in places where we would not expect the preposition to in spoken English.
- (80)
- a.
- going vacation next week, but i’m ugly with nothing packed
- b.
- going places
- c.
- going home for Christmas
- d.
- we going someplace to eat before the show starts
The spoken English preposition in (80a) would be ‘on’, and the words ‘places’, ‘home’, and ‘someplace’ in these examples would be introduced by a null preposition in spoken English and are ungrammatical with an overt preposition (Collins 2007; Schoenmakers & Storment 2021).
This construction is very common on the internet, even having its own Urban Dictionary entry. Grammaticalization creating prepositions is nothing new: ‘behind’, ‘betwixt’, ‘before’, ‘between’, ‘in front of’, just to name a few in English. If emojis can be lexically represented, there is no reason that they could not be grammaticalized as well. The connection between the iconicity of the airplane and a directional preposition is not hard to see. It is far more intuitive than using any other emoji, say , as a directional preposition.
Another interesting example of a pro-text emoji being used as a functional category element among young people on Twitter is the emoji as a second person singular pronoun.
- (81)
- a.
- I’m looking at
- b.
- It’s simple… If engage with me, I’ll engage with
- c.
- Don’t worry about what others are doing. Worry about what are doing.
This example’s iconicity is pretty straightforward, as the interpretation of the emoji comes from the fact that it is pointing at the addressee (the reader). This is in fact the same symbol as the sign for the second person singular pronoun in ASL. This is another example of an expressive symbol becoming a pro-text emoji, only now it is a pronoun.
Pro-text emojis, then, can belong to functional categories as well as lexical ones. These are the only examples of such a thing known to me at the time, which should not be shocking given that at this point emojis are only eleven years old, and these language change processes take time. In the future we should expect to see more cases of grammaticalization of pro-text emojis.
6 A Note on Representation
At this point it is crucial to ask not only what pro-text emojis represent morphosyntactically, but what the nature of the difference is between pro-text emojis and regular English orthography (or speech, for that matter). There is a body of experimental work suggesting a difference in the way that emojis are processed from surrounding text (Cohn et al. 2018; Barach et al. 2021). This of course could simply be due to the difference in modality (Cohn & Schilperoord 2022). As discussed in Section 4.4., since pro-text emojis do not refer to any phonological form in the way that standard orthography does, they are phonetically underspecified and lexically ambiguous. It is not straightforward the ways in which emojis make reference to elements in the lexicon. Not all emojis need to be associated with a unique, novel lexical representation, but surely some can be for some language users (see in Section 5.1.). This is reinforced by the fact that, among young people who use these emojis frequently online, it is not uncommon to hear sentences such as the following actually spoken in verbal communication.10
- (82)
- a.
- I’m so demon emoji rn
- b.
- I feel so pleading emoji right now
- c.
- I’m like skull emoji but words
Therefore, it is important to identify the ways in which pro-text emojis represent lexical items, if and when they cannot be said to represent a novel form unique to the emoji. Based on the data available in this paper and on Twitter, at least two separate patterns emerge. In one, pro-text emojis obligatorily make reference to preexisting words In the other, the pro-text emoji may simply be interpreted iconically, without making reference to any other lexical item to be understood, by way of its visual similarity to what it represents (Davidson 2015). I dub these hybrid emojis and iconic emojis, respectively, borrowing terminology from (Greenberg 2023). In this section I discuss details and examples of these two potential categories, as well as some basic diagnostics for differentiating the two. Up until now this paper has not distinguished between these two categories as the morphosyntactic generalizations of pro-text emojis hold up of both categories. The morphosyntactic generalizations laid out in this paper still hold of both hybrid and iconic emojis. I suggest this dichotomy as a way of thinking about the variation of the mechanisms in which emojis make reference to a language user’s lexicon.
6.1 Hybrid Emojis
As stated above, the interpretation of hybrid emojis relies on the language user’s ability to make an association between the pro-text emoji and a related lexical item, and retrieve the meaning that way. The name hybrid refers to the mechanism of interpretation of this class of pro-speech emojis being partially iconic and partially symbolic (Greenberg 2023). See the following examples.
- (83)
- ’ed him bc I’m tryna get in the Halloween spirit [int: ghosted]
- (84)
- my eyes are so dilated it looks like i’ve been snorting nonstop [int: ketamine]
Here the emojis are not just being interpreted iconically and are instead a reference to the idiomatic meanings of the English verb ‘ghost’ and noun ‘ketamine’. They are, of course, being interpreted iconically. For example, the language user must first identify the emoji in (83) as a picture of a ghost. The user must then make the symbolic association from the depiction of the form of a ghost to the interpretation of the verb ghost meaning “to cease to respond to (a person) on social media, by text message, etc.” (OED 2023b). A test to show that these emojis have a mechanism of interpretation beyond that which they iconically denote is that they are not available to use in this way in languages that lack similar lexical items. For example, in Chinese, the equivalent to the verb ‘ghost’ in English, meaning to cut off communication (often of a romantic nature) with someone suddenly and without explanation, would be 鸽 ‘to pigeon’. See the following example from a native speaker informant.
- (85)
- 他
- he
- 了
- pigeon-perf
- 我
- me
- ‘He pigeoned (ghosted) me’
Note that swapping the pro-text emojis in (83) and (85) would yield uninterpretable results in both English and Chinese, as their interpretations rely upon the availability of the associations between similar lexical items in each language.
A similar point can be demonstrated language-internally, as well. Take example (20c) of this paper, repeated below.
- (86)
- a couple of s smoking s [int: fags; fags]
The original Tweet here contains a picture of two (presumably gay) men in the bathtub together smoking cigarettes. The appropriate reading of the sentence, then, is a couple of fags smoking fags. Preliminary judgment tasks confirm that this particular sentence is very difficult for people to parse who are not aware that some gay men use the word fag as an in-group self-identifier, or that the word fag in British English means cigarette. This means that the interpretation of the first emoji has several steps. First the language user must identify must identify the emoji as a picture of a cigarette. The user must then make the association between the picture and the British lexical item fag, then the American English lexical item fag meaning a male homosexual, and that this is the how the emoji is intended to be interpreted by the person who wrote the Tweet, which is suggested by the accompanying picture, and more generally the context of being on ‘gay Twitter’.
Another example from queer subculture is the emoji , which is often used by queer people online to refer to the camp aesthetic. An example of this is shown below.
- (87)
- I’m tired of pretending to not like riverdale, it’s [int: camp]
This is a clear instance of the emoji referring to ironic bad taste, as opposed to the many instances of people who are not part of subcultures that regularly discuss the camp aesthetic using the emoji to refer to actual tents and campsites.
Since the interpretation of hybrid emojis relies on making associations between preexisting words, we would expect these emojis to always have an accessible phonological form, so if a person were asked to read a sentence containing a hybrid emoji aloud, they would not struggle to find an equivalent spoken word for the emoji, which intuitively seems to be the case, though experimental results to demonstrate this will be valuable. We would also expect hybrid emojis to be less likely to display the morphological regularization seen in (62–63) of this paper, as there could be phonology associated with hybrid emojis available to the language user. These examples are highly reminiscent of – if not identical to – Rebuses, which are symbols that are phonetic approximations to the target word.
- (88)
- eye
- can
- sea
- yew
- ‘I can see you’
With Rebuses, there is no clear iconic association between the pictures and the target interpretation. The pictures only serve as phonetic approximations of the target word (eye and I both having the phonetic form /ai/). When reading the Rebus in (88), the picture must first be identified as an eye (iconic interpretation), then the association must be made between the lexical items eye and I. Often times, the intended interpretations of a Rebus is not clear without looking at the entire sentence. In these cases the interaction between the iconic mechanism of interpretation – which is identifying the pictures – and the symbolic mechanism of interpretation – which is making associations between existing lexical items and interpreting them in within an utterance – are clear and both necessary to interpreting hybrid emojis.
6.2 Iconic Emojis
Iconic emojis are only interpreted iconically, even if they denote a highly specific object/action. There does not need to be a further symbolic mechanism of lexical association and retrieval to understand this class of pro-text emoji. Often times there is no salient phonetic form of an iconic emoji, which can lead to confusion when a person is asked to read aloud a sentence containing an iconic emoji (as seen with in Section 5.1.). Despite not always having a clear phonetic form, it is generally not the case that iconic emojis are more difficult to understand. In fact, due to the lack of a need to form an association with a different lexical item, they may be more straightforward to interpret.
This is not to say, however, that every iconic emoji cannot have a phonetic approximation. Take the following example.
- (89)
- I my s [int: love; cats]
Few – if any – people who have seen emojis before would struggle to read this sentence out loud, and it is sufficient to say that this is because the semantic denotations of and are almost identical to those of the noun cat and the verb love. Although, interestingly, the verb represented by the iconic emoji has gained a distinct pronunciation, which is heart. Preliminary results would suggest that a similar number of people would read (89) as ‘I heart my cats’ to those who would read it as ‘I love my cats’. This phenomenon of an emoji gaining a novel pronunciation is also something that would only be expected with iconic emojis, not hybrid emojis, as hybrid emojis have a specified phonetic form already. The novel phonetic form may suggest that the iconic emoji represents a distinct lexical item from the verb love.
Cases in which there is an emoji with no obvious phonetic form and a clear iconic interpretation, such as discussed in Section 5.1., are unambiguously iconic emojis. It cannot be the case that this emoji’s interpretation relies on the knowledge of another lexical item because there is no other lexical item in English with a meaning that can be roughly represented by the following.
- (90)
- ∃e[making the facial expression(e) & Agent(e, x) & Goal (e,y)]
A major question raised by the dichotomy of hybrid emojis and iconic emojis is whether or not iconic emojis uniquely represent elements in a person’s lexicon. Obviously, it is not ideal to posit that every iconic emoji represents a distinct entry in a person’s lexicon, just as it is not ideal to posit that any possible pro-speech gesture in spoken language is represented in someone’s lexicon, though certainly some gestures are lexically represented; however, there are instances where iconic emojis can come to represent something in the language user’s lexicon that is only represented by the pro-text emoji. See and above.
Greenberg (2023) establishes iconic representations and symbolic representation as a spectrum, with iconic representations including pictures and symbolic representations including linguistic expressions. The discussion of representation of pro-text emojis in this paper supports the concept of iconicity-symbolism as a spectrum, with hybrid emojis falling closer on the spectrum to being symbolic than iconic emojis. It is interesting to consider that emojis themselves are often quite conventionalized, for example the emoji is used in lieu of the emoji above, which is closer to a depictively iconic (Davidson 2015) representation of a heart, so it is possible that even iconic emojis have some symbolism in their interpretation. Hybrid emojis, though, must be interpreted both iconically and symbolically, whereas iconic emojis may be interpreted solely as denoting what the picture represents. This means that, while all pro-text emojis represent morphological stems, the mechanisms by which they represent any given lexical item can vary in terms of the degree of iconicity and symbolism involved in the interpretation.
Davidson (2023) distinguishes between two types of iconicity: lexical and descriptive. With lexical iconicity, the meaning of a form is not entirely arbitrary, but still has to be retrieved using a conventionalized form–meaning mapping. With depictive iconicity, meaning is not mediated by the lexicon, but rather the form maps directly to meaning via direct non-symbolic depiction. An example of this would be the English word chirp being lexically iconic, while whistling to imitate a bird call is depictively iconic. Perhaps, then, the interpretation of some iconic pro-speech emoji stems is lexically iconic, while the interpretation of others is depictively iconic. This also supports the idea of gradience between iconicity and symbolism, as lexically iconic emojis become are more integrated into a symbolic system than depictively iconic ones.
7 Conclusion
In this paper I demonstrate that there is a distinct class of emojis – pro-text emojis – which represent independent morphological stems that take word-level affixes, which, in English, include all inflectional affixes and outer-layer derivational affixes. Data supporting these facts are incredibly numerous, well-attested, and confirmed by the intuitions of native speakers who are proficient with emoji use online. The existence of some of these pro-text emojis can also be analyzed as a result of lexicalization and grammaticalization.
Within pro-text emojis, two potential classes have been identified: hybrid emojis, whose interpretations rely on the meaning of another lexical item, and iconic emojis, whose meaning is interpreted pictorially (Greenberg 2012).
This research builds on current morphological theory by identifying how a class of pictorial symbols interact with affixation and demonstrating that these elements must be morphological stems. As these elements can be lexically represented, this contributes to the body of evidence that stems are what is represented in a language user’s lexicon. It also supports the concept of an iconicity-symbolism spectrum in which elements of different modalities with varying degrees of iconicity can interact in the same linguistic system.
This work builds on ideas introduced in the super-linguistics program, namely that expressive elements can be analyzed in a formal linguistic system (Greenberg 2021; Schlenker 2019). My work here demonstrates that symbolic elements actually can be part of the linguistic system themselves, where they conform to regular morphosyntactic principles. This makes a much-needed connection with work on the semantics of emojis (Grosz & Kaiser & Pierini 2021; Pierini 2021; Grosz et al. 2022; Pasternak & Tieu 2022; Maier 2023) to morphology, and by extension syntax (Collins & Kayne 2021).
This opens up the possibility of looking more closely at the morphology and syntax of other pro-speech elements such as gestures or even music (Migotti & Guerrini 2023). Prior research (Bruening 2018a; b; Müller 2018) discusses to some extent the phenomenon of non-linguistic elements being incorporated as words into a linguistic system; however, specific examples of this have not been looked at in great detail before. Additionally, I make the novel proposal that some pro-text emojis represent completely unique stems and therefore represent unique lexical items. This paper explores a specific example of this phenomenon, shedding light on the structural positions these elements can occupy as well as how they are represented in the mind of the language user, and how they may form part of a user’s lexicon.
Acknowledgements
There are many people to thank for this project. First, the users of Twitter and Tumblr – especially those users from marginalized communities who show incredible innovation in language despite prescriptive pressure from the dominant culture – for inspiring this project in the first place with their amazing language use online, as well as the many informants who provide insightful grammaticality judgments for these data. Second, I would like to thank the members of my second QP committee at Stony Brook University where this project was first defended: John Bailyn, Richard Larson, Jiwon Yun, and Jenny Singleton. I would also like to thank Masha Esipova for showing interest in this project and allowing me to present my work at her Omni-linguistics course at NYI. A big thanks to Hagen Blix, Thomas Graf, and Owen Rambow for help with the German glosses and emoji interpretations. I would also like to thank Mark Aronoff for our conversations about roots, and Natalie Thomey for our conversations about emojis. I lastly want to thank the three anonymous reviewers at Glossa, as well as the editor, Johan Rooryck, for their incredibly helpful feedback.
Competing interests
The author has no competing interests to declare.
Notes
- It should be noted that the example “I NY” predates emojis as defined in this paper by several decades. Nevertheless, the example is emblematic of the way emojis are used as grammatical elements in online communication, and is indicative of the fact that this phenomenon is not necessarily limited only to the set of symbols on the emoji digital keyboard as it is known today. [^]
- I do not have exact numbers on how many languages have speakers that use or accept pro-text emojis, but I assume they are possible in any language whose speakers have access to emojis. [^]
- For each English example containing pro-text emojis, I give an approximate translation in brackets of what the emoji means. Examples in sentences with more than one pro-text emoji are separated by semicolons. [^]
- Despite being renamed X, I continue to refer to X as Twitter and to X-posts as Tweets. [^]
- Many speakers will add an apostrophe between an emoji and an affix. This seems to be nothing more than an arbitrary stylistic choice. [^]
- Here it is worth mentioning if zero morphology is necessary to promote a bare lexically-typed stem to a freestanding word, for example English singular nouns or first person present indicative verbs (Nida 1948; Dahl & Fábregas 2018). I believe that this is the case, though I do not think a full discussion of the theoretical implications of this is entirely relevant for this specific discussion. [^]
- There are several flower emojis: , , , , etc. All are unattested with the suffix, and changing one emoji for another does not improve the acceptability of the example. [^]
- The video itself can be viewed here: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=G2a9c240WVM. A discussion of the virality of the meme can be viewed here: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Kn_MNGgxT6Q. [^]
- This informant is also the person who explained to me the cultural background of the lobster suicide meme. [^]
- These examples are taken from Twitter but indicate the general phenomenon I am describing in spoken language as well. Certainly the fact that these sentences appear in CMC when the option to simply use the emoji being described is available as well is incredibly interesting. [^]
References
Abner, Natasha & Cooperrider, Kensy & Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2015. Gesture for linguists: A handy primer. Language and Linguistics Compass 9(11). 437–451. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12168
Alexiadou, Artemis. 2004. Inflection class, gender and DP-internal structure. In Müller, Gereon & Gunkel, Lutz & Zifonun, Gisela (eds.), Explorations in Nominal Inflection, 21–50. Berlin: Mouton. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197501.21
Al-Rashdi, Fathiya. 2018. Functions of emojis in WhatsApp interaction among Omanis. Discourse, Context & Media. 26. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcm.2018.07.001
Anderson, Stephen. 1992. A-morphous Morphology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511586262
Aronoff, Mark. 1976. Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aronoff, Mark. 1994. Morphology by Itself: Stems and Inflectional Classes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aronoff, Mark & Sridhar, Shikaripur. 1983. Morphological levels in English and Kannada. In Richardson, John & Mitchell, Marks & Chukerman, Amy (eds.), Papers from the Parasession on the Interplay of Phonology, Morphology, and Syntax 3–16. Chicago, IL, USA: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Bai, Qiyu & Dan, Qi & Mu, Zhe & Yang, Maokun. 2019. A systematic review of emoji: Current research and future perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology 10. DOI: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02221
Barach, Eliza & Feldman, Laurie Beth & Sheridan, Heather. 2021. Are emojis processed like words?: Eye movements reveal the time course of semantic processing for emojified text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28. DOI: http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01864-y
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 2013. The Spanish lexicon stores stems with theme vowels, not roots with inflectional features. Probus 25. 3–103. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/probus-2013-0009
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 2018. Stratal phonology. In Hannahs, S. J. & Bosch, Anna (eds.), The Routledge handbook of phonological theory, 100–134. London, UK: Routledge. DOI: http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315675428-5
Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Holt.
Bruening, Benjamin. 2018a. The lexicalist hypothesis: Both wrong and superfluous. Language 94(1). 1–42. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2018.0000
Bruening, Benjamin. 2018b. Brief response to Müller. Language 94(1). 67–73. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2018.0015
Burge, Jeremy. 2020. A New King: Pleading Face. Emojipedia. Blog. https://blog.emojipedia.org/a-new-king-pleading-face/
Campbell, Lyle & Janda, Richard. 2000. Conceptions of grammaticalization and their problems. Language Sciences 23(2–3). 93–112. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/S0388-0001(00)00018-8
Chung, Debbie. 2020. What is Stan Twitter? — how it started and how it’s going? Medium. https://medium.com/@debbiechungxinhui/what-is-stan-twitter-how-it-started-and-how-its-going-ad849706ee07
Cohn, Neil. 2012. Explaining ‘I can’t draw’: Parallels between the structure and development of language and drawing. Human Development 55(4). 167–192. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1159/000341842
Cohn, Neil & Engelen, Jan & Schilperoord, Joost. 2019. The grammar of emoji? Constraints on communicative pictorial sequencing. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications 4(33). DOI: http://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-019-0177-0
Cohn, Neil & Roijackers, Tim & Schaap, Robin & Engelen, Jan. (2018). Are emoji a poor substitute for words? Sentence processing with emoji substitutions. In Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 1524–1529. The Cognitive Science Society.
Cohn, Neil & Schilperoord, Joost. 2022. Remarks on multimodality: Grammatical interactions in the parallel architecture. Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence 4. DOI: http://doi.org/10.3389/frai.2021.778060
Collins, Chris. 2007. Home sweet home. Manuscript. NYU.
Collins, Chris. 2019. Two Kinds of Data in Syntactic Fieldwork: Experimental and Non-Experimental. Ordinary Working Grammarian. Blog. https://ordinaryworkinggrammarian.blogspot.com/2019/12/two-kinds-of-data-in-syntactic.html
Collins, Chris & Kayne, Richard. 2021. Towards a theory of morphology as syntax. Manuscript. Lingbuzz.
Curtis, Charles. 2017. Just when did we all start using GOAT anyway? For The Win. USA Today. Blog. https://www.usatoday.com/story/sports/ftw/2017/08/04/just-when-did-we-all-start-using-goat-anyway/104289206/
Dahl, Eystein & Fábregas, Antonio. 2018. Zero morphemes. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.592
Danesi, Marcel. 2017. The Semiotics of Emoji. London: Bloomsbury Academic. DOI: http://doi.org/10.5040/9781474282024
Davidson, Kathryn. 2015. Quotation, demonstration, and iconicity. Linguistics and Philosophy 38(6). 477–520. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-015-9180-1
Davidson, Kathryn. 2023. Semiotic distinctions in compositional semantics. Proceedings of the 58th Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society.
Embick, David & Marantz, Alec. 2008. Architecture and Blocking. Linguistic Inquiry 39(1). 1–53. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1162/ling.2008.39.1.1
Emojipedia. 2024. https://emojipedia.org
Esipova, Maria. 2019. Composition and projection of co-speech gestures. Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory 29. 117137. DOI: http://doi.org/10.3765/salt.v29i0.4600
Evans, Vyvyan. 2017. The Emoji code: The linguistics behind smiley faces and scaredy cats. MacMillan Picador.
Fahlman, Scott. 1982. Smiley Lore :-). Carnegie Melon University. Blog. https://www.cs.cmu.edu/~sef/sefSmiley.htm
Gawne, Lauren & McCulloch, Gretchen. 2019. Emoji as digital gestures. Language @ Internet.
Giegerich, Heinz. 1999. Lexical Strata in English: Morphological Causes, Phonological Effects. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511486470
Giegerich, Heiz. 2005. Lexicalism and modular overlap in English. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics 2(2). 43–62.
Goldin-Meadow, Susan & Brentari, Diane. 2017. Gesture, sign, and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies. The Behavioral and brain sciences 40(46). DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1600039X
Greenberg, Gabriel. 2012. Pictorial Semantics. Manuscript. UCLA.
Greenberg, Gabriel. 2021. Semantics of Pictorial Space. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 1(4). 847–887. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-020-00513-6
Greenberg, Gabriel. 2023. The iconic-symbolic spectrum. Manuscript. UCLA. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1215/00318108-10697558
Grosz, Patrick & Greenberg, Gabriel & de Leon, Christina & Kaiser, Elsi. 2022. A semantics of face emoji in discourse. Manuscript. Lingbuzz. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-022-09369-8
Grosz, Patrick & Kaiser, Elsi & Pierini, Francesco. 2021. Discourse anaphoricity and first-person indexicality in emoji resolution. Manuscript.
Hilpert, Martin. 2019. Lexicalization in Morphology. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics, 1–18. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.622
Hunter, Julie. 2019. Relating Gesture to Speech: reflections on the role of conditional presuppositions. Linguistics and Philosophy 42, 317–332. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-018-9244-0
Ishmael, Aiyana. 2021. Sending Smiley Emojis? They Now Mean Different Things to Different People. Wall Street Journal. https://www.wsj.com/articles/sending-a-smiley-face-make-sure-you-know-what-youre-saying-11628522840
Kastovsky, Dieter. 1994. Typological differences between English and German morphology and their causes. In Swan, Toril & Mørck, Endre & Jansen, Olaf (eds.), Language Change and Language Structure: Older Germanic Languages in a Comparative Perspective, 135–158. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110886573.135
Kiparsky, Paul. 2015. Stratal OT: A synopsis and FAQs. Manuscript. Stanford University.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2020. Morphological Units: Stems. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.542
Knudson, Annalise. 2017. Health department STD campaign uses emoji lingo. Staten Island Live. https://www.silive.com/news/2017/09/city_health_department_std_cam.html
Kramer, Ruth. 2016. The location of gender features in the syntax. Language and Linguistics Compass 10, 661–677. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12226
Kress, Gunther. 2009. Multimodality: A Social Semiotic Approach to Contemporary Communication. Rutledge.
Ladislaides, Ján. 1635. Financial Record. State Archives. Trenčin, Slovakia.
Lasnik, Howard. 1995. Verbal morphology: Syntactic Structures meets the minimalist program. In Campos, Héctor & Kempchinsky, Paula (eds.) Evolution and Revolution in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Carlos Otero, 251–275. Georgetown University Press.
Leondis, Tony. 2017. The Emoji Movie. Film.
Lieber, Rochelle. 1976. On the Organization of the Lexicon. Doctoral Dissertation. MIT.
Lohndal, Terje. 2020. Syntactic Categorization of Roots. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.257
Maier, Emar. 2023. Emojis as Pictures. Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy 10(11). DOI: http://doi.org/10.3998/ergo.4641
Marantz Alec. 2007. Phases and words. In Choe, Sook-Hee (ed.), Phases in the Theory of Grammar. 199–222. Seoul, South Korea: Dong-In Publishing Co.
Migotti, Léo & Guerrini, Janek. 2023. Linguistic Inferences from Pro-Speech Music. Linguistics and Philosophy 46(4). 989–1026. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-022-09376-9
Moschini, Ilaria. 2016. The “Face with Tears of Joy” Emoji. A Socio-Semiotic and Multimodal Insight into a Japan-America Mash-Up. HERMES – Journal of Language and Communication in Business 55, 11–25. DOI: http://doi.org/10.7146/hjlcb.v0i55.24286
Müller, Stefan. 2018. The end of lexicalism as we know it? Language 94(1). e54–e66. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2018.0014
Nida, Eugene. 1948. The identification of morphemes. Language 24(4). 414–441. DOI: http://doi.org/10.2307/410358
Olsen, Susan. 1986. Wortbildung im Deutschen. Eine Einführung in die Theorie der Wortstruktur. Kröners Studienbibliothek 660. Stuttgart: Kröner.
Oxford English Dictionary (OED). 2023a. Emoji, N. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/OED/6435531294
Oxford English Dictionary (OED). 2023b. Ghost, V. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/OED/9179941513
Pasternak, Robert & Tieu, Lyn. 2022. Co-linguistic content inferences: From gestures to sound effects and emoji. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 75(10), 1828–1843. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1177/17470218221080645
Perrin, Andrew & Atske, Sara. 2021. About three-in-ten U.S. adults say they are ‘almost constantly’ online. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/short-reads/2021/03/26/about-three-in-ten-u-s-adults-say-they-are-almost-constantly-online/
Picallo, M. Carme. 1991. Nominals and nominalization in Catalan. Probus 3, 279–316. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/prbs.1991.3.3.279
Pierini, Francesco. 2021. Emojis and gestures: a new typology. Proceedings of Sinn Und Bedeutung 25, 720–732. DOI: http://doi.org/10.18148/sub/2021.v25i0.963
Roberts, Ian & Roussou, Anna. 2003. Syntactic Change: A Minimalist Approach to Grammaticalization. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511486326
Sapir, Edward. 1925. Sound patterns in language. Language 1(2). 37–51. DOI: http://doi.org/10.2307/409004
Schlenker, Philippe. 2017. Iconic Pragmatics. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 36(3). 877–936. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-017-9392-x
Schlenker, Philippe. 2018. Gestural Semantics. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 37(2). 735–784. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-018-9414-3
Schlenker, Philippe. 2019. What is Super Semantics? Philosophical Perspectives 32(6). DOI: http://doi.org/10.1111/phpe.12122
Schoenmakers, Gert-Jan & Storment, John David. 2021. Going city: Directional predicates and preposition incorporation in youth vernaculars of Dutch. Linguistics in the Netherlands 38(1). 65–80. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1075/avt.00050.sch
Scott, Carol & Bay-Cheng, Laina & Prince, Mark & Nochajski, Thomas & Collins, R. Lorraine. 2017. Time spent online: Latent profile analyses of emerging adults’ social media use. Computers in Human Behavior 75. 311–319. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.05.026
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1982. The Syntax of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Siegel, Dorothy Carla. 1974. Topics in English Morphology. Doctoral dissertation. MIT.
Stahl, Levi. 2014. The First Emoticon? I’ve Been Reading Lately. Blog. http://ivebeenreadinglately.blogspot.com/2014/04/the-first-emoticon.html
Thelin, Nils. (1973). On stem formation, conjugation and accentuation of the Russian verb. Scando-Slavica 19(1). 83–102. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1080/00806767308600629
Tieu, Lyn & Qiu, Jimmy & Puvipalan, Vaishnavy & Pasternak, Robert. 2023. Experimental evidence for a semantic typology of emoji: Inferences of co-, pro-, and post-text emoji. Manuscript. Lingbuzz.
Uriagereka, Juan & Gallegos, Ángel. 2023. Interclausal Dependencies. Manuscript.
Votruba, Martin. 2018. 17th Century Emoji. Slovak Studies Program. University of Pittsburgh. https://web.archive.org/web/20180810182423/http://www.pitt.edu/~votruba/qsonhist/smileyoldestslovakia.html
Wiese, Richard. 1996. Phrasal compounds and the theory of word syntax. Linguistic Inquiry, 27(1). 189–193.
WOWPresents. 2015. Katya Zamo & Trixie Mattel – Bestie$ for Ca$h. YouTube video. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=RsnrYtTTocY
Zwicky, Arnold. 1992. Some choices in the theory of morphology. In Formal grammar: Theory and Implementation, 327–321. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195073102.003.0006
Zulier, Nino Giuliano. 2021. Conceptualization of a Queer Cyberspace: ‘Gay Twitter’. Freiburger Zeitschrift für Geschlechterstudien 27(1). 95–111. DOI: http://doi.org/10.3224/fzg.v27i1.07